A _
HH 7 H
=
= SSE
SS
i
=e
Ha HUNK ui -
iH i i a i
a i Hi
SS SS
8
Sa
a
SS
if Mi
a
i 7
=
eee
~ Ae eee
= ——
Sereseste
———
i i ;
i H i \ HH
: : i : i:
ae Hl
i [ i — a
au
ate 7 -
i : 7 y 7
i ee a
. a a ae i | i a i
oo ” io
a
if 7 :
an i |
na Wal i on Ha | ,
at | .
ite (
| at v i: a
ey i i
: i i : i
=
See
—— oe =
———— See
ee
SS
: = a
TS SSS SE
SS
)
: o
a
Hh
a i
ni i
a
a
:
ith
ae
Hl
%
f
4
|
.
SSeS
SS
ee
See
at aes
i a
ou Hi | i | |
}
H vi 4)
i a Wi | :
a }
| ae iy a
|
Repeat ae
———
HESS
/ i
= Serer
Sse
SSS sae
S SSS:
Saoees
ely
Si: Se SSeS SSS
a
HARVARD UNIVERSITY
Sele aei
I TASIE
LIBRARY
OF THE
Museum of Comparative Zoology
pita
MUS. COMP. ZOOL.
LIBRARY | |
JAN 27 iyo joc
_ HARVARD —
_ UNIVERSITY, :
\
<i
—
THE NATURAL HISTORY AND
BEHAVIOR OF THE CALIFORNIA
SEA LION
SPECIAL PUBLICATIONS
This series, published by the American Society of Mammalogists,
has been established for papers of monographic scope concerned
with some aspect of the biology of mammals.
Correspondence concerning manuscripts to be submitted for
publication in the series should be addressed to the chairman (and
editor) of the Committee on Special Publications.
Copies of special publications may be ordered from the Secretary-
treasurer of the Society, Dr. Bryan P. Glass, Department of Zoology,
Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, Oklahoma 74075.
Price of this issue $3.50
COMMITTEE ON SPECIAL PUBLICATIONS
WILLIAM H. Burt, Chairman and Editor
Museum of Zoology,
University of Michigan,
Ann Arbor, Michigan 48104.
J. Knox Jones, Jr.
Museum of Natural History,
University of Kansas,
Lawrence, Kansas 66044.
JAMEs N. LAyNE
Division of Biological Sciences,
Cornell University,
Ithaca, New York 14850.
M. RAyMonpD LEE
Museum of Natural History,
University of Illinois,
Urbana, Illinois 61803.
SMOO PUR [[Nq [LILO}III9}
:SUOT] BIS BIULOJI[ED
ee
1V
THE NATURAL HISTORY AND
BEHAVIOR OF THE
CALIFORNIA SEA LION
By
RICHARD S. PETERSON
Division OF NATURAL SCIENCES,
UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SANTA CRUZ
AND
GEORGE A. BARTHOLOMEW
DEPARTMENT OF ZOOLOGY
UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, Los ANGELES
SPECIAL PUBLICATION NO. 1
‘THE AMERICAN SOCIETY OF MAMMALOGISTS
PUBLISHED DECEMBER 5, 1967
MUS. COMP. ZOOL.
LIBRARY
JAN 27 Ses
“HARVARD
UNIVERSITY,
Library of Congress Catalog Card No. 67-29225
© 1967 by The American Society of Mammalogists
v1
FOREWORD
N 1939 Seth B. Benson, then assistant curator of mammals at the
Museum of Vertebrate Zoology of the University of California
in Berkeley, remarked to one of us (G.A.B.) that a natural history
study of the California sea lion was long overdue. Such a study was
even longer overdue by the spring of 1965 when the intensive
phase of our observations on this species was begun. For some
inexplicable reason, during the more than quarter of a century
which has elapsed since Benson’s remark no detailed field study
of the behavior of these most accessible of marine mammals has
been published.
This neglect is remarkable in view of the wide spread interest
in marine mammals which has flourished since World War II. Dur-
ing this period many zoologists, including the authors, have devoted
much time and effort to field studies of pinnipeds in the Arctic,
the Antarctic, and on remote islands, but the many thousands of
sea lions which breed on the islands just off the populous coasts of
southern California have been neglected. It is remarkable that the
only extensive publication on the general behavior of Zalophus
californianus is based on the isolated tropical population, Z. c. wol-
lebaekt, which I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt studied in the Galapagos Islands.
We hope that our work will help to fill in an anomalous gap in
the knowledge of pinnipeds, and that it will supply a background
of natural history against which current and future laboratory and
experimental studies of California sea lions can be interpreted. The
present study is obviously little more than a first step toward under-
standing the social behavior of the California sea lion. Much re-
mains to be done, particularly the quantification of information and
the analysis of population dynamics. Nevertheless, even though
our findings are incomplete, they should be of value as a point of
departure for subsequent and more sophisticated analyses.
Our work on Z. californianus included the recording and physical
analysis of the major patterns of vocalization in this highly vocal
species, but only the more general aspects of these data are included
here. The details will be published separately elsewhere.
As is the case of most field studies a number of persons and insti-
tutions contributed directly and indirectly to our research. Our
field work was made possible by a grant (MH-11430-01) from the
Vil
National Institutes of Health of the U. S. Public Health Service,
and by the assistance of the Marine Sciences Division of the Life
Sciences Department, U. S. Naval Missile ‘Test Center, Point Mugu,
California. We are grateful for the support of Dr. C. H. Maag and
Mr. F. G. Wood, Jr., of the Life Sciences Department, and of Cdr.
H. R. Megrew USN and other officers and men on San Nicolas
Island. We thank H. J. Chiellon, H. N. Coulombe, and E. C. Beers
for their assistance in the field, and W. E. Evans and W. Ross for
generously making field equipment available to us. Mr. C. H.
Fiscus of the U. S. Fish and Wildlife Service has allowed us to
include unpublished data from his pelagic studies-of sea lions. Part
of the data analysis and writing was done while one of us (R.S.P.)
was a National Science Foundation Postdoctoral Fellow at the
Bureau of Animal Population, Oxford University, and the other of
us (G.A.B.) was aided by a grant (GB-966) from the National
Science Foundation.
Oxford and Los Angeles,
June, 1966.
Vill
Contents
PAGE
Tarai tate CLUE tel © rena EEC Lee en ere 1
“ICGTRRNTTG CER eee ee. eee 2
IMUQDACOIG ste a Eh eee ee eee: Se eee 2
(Geimeralll TOM oye ee ee ee ee 4
IN@IMIKE DIROGUINCHNNS. SIME Ore ee ee 6
Vhs(@x CKO MAGNETS OU ele se ac! ces a 6
Individual Mannerisms on Land __-...------- 8
Sy XXCTGIT SYS OS ae ae ee ee 11
Interspecific Sociallleeelati@ rise seater ral ove eeen oes ae A 14
INon=reproductive: social Mimteractioms =e 18
Ieproductivie be WaviOn 2: =e Seer 2)
INA male Gran tO TGS pees ee Oe eels Gay ne Oe eee eT OY
iBehaviorotebemalles = ee ee ee 28
Aa GUUTA 1 OTe eee is ae Raney belo A hte Wes ne eee te eR 31
Countshiprands Copulation, =a an 32
Mother-Young Relations and Ontogeny of Pup Behavior __ 35
Reninatalebeliavione st Sed ee ee 35
1 PGT Ol BNE] GENO) ene eee i ene ew a ee eee ee 37
Mother-Young Recognition and Suckling — 40
Quantifiable Aspects of the Biology of Zalophus —.____- 45
Ro pulatirome) yanarii Cs, See ee 45
Behaviorcob olin diwvacivall si ee 49
DD TSGUISST@ epee eens seinen ne EN) ha corte beeen ales 58
(GSE OTIS Yr ee ere 58
ENE ISIN SAN SIS i A ee ere re 59
ANTM TUMOO WIS ANGI ONO 63
JEWOlGOMN OP Waite Soll CUS se 66
SUL A May gee eae ein EN Pelee ee Sk conte eg tae be = 70
INE eee reea HUTT ee GG Clg ash we a Ag a ih el ah ls 73
Va GUE8e ann koe hs a es ae Ss Ree es ee 77
il
a 5 I)
' 7 ?
™ 7
ns
:
‘
aries! iGayeteiey. Ga ‘isan oe on fi - al He
chun a ea a. | Hing hha em Phone Cr eutiag
toe thy t A Rial Whi or Cab
| ond? om, ATS Cora ve Se ri ao Wie
his, We fe PMS TRE? ree 4 Ai’ Sr ee
i } Wade’ itty. ear ah de ce! Suey ete
+p) %, ov). eee." Ss vier
, ig
; tea "HA en
ripe Or Lh at en ee a eevee ts ie
Pe? 9 is Pe Dat) ine Bae ” (ae cana ’
ms ianh J pafthughrnr tt Tl Geviredi wlnleia
| | ~ :
; if ya, 9) oy P
" i}
Pa ’ a
i , v
cal i
vy
1
7
" fi
i
a i}
i ae
P
X
¢
~
‘Ty;
-
APTe
=
6
=
Illustrations
TEYROWOVNS ONS <a ee ee iv
FIGURE FACING PAGE
IS tudy arcavon) san! Nicolas Island) == 25s ae SS 8
2. Jeuyo wierd aol oye nenie Cll o) oni er ae 9
B, leanne syd @hy Cattle ae 9
A) Bow-and-artow marking, technique = Se 16
5. Female and pup moving from one territory to another ___ 16
6. Prostrate sleeping postures (ONE SOU DSc a ee 17
(eetemale sleepine any vertical post une me ee 17
oe IPUID Somes Cope nO i 2 20
9, Wersmoparall lwill Cobras wae We 21
10. Bull defending area adjacent to dead female —_______- 21
I, TR@ Lolly \eatan (eetermfeareves iim \Wyelere os
IZaaSpatialvarancement of bulls a 25
IS. Browranceiny Geieecnoyay ON WUC, 28
Ieee INCRO DIG MEAS Cal Cy serene: eo caterer sence tee Was ieee ea ee 29
lbeelevateds posturemwith: head-shaking = sss 32
lowe Open-mouchsthneatssbyetemales see 33
IVAN CCKED Uo lity ee ree ee Ae oe oak EE IS 36
IQs: (Chay ouulleiohe7 4 ronnie eee ee ee eee 36
NG ees US eureslralll OW Wee Ty = ease semen oe eee 37
7A () Sates Focus Soaps cay =f fn 1 ree 2 a ae Be ee 44
2 Female SUGISTTI GS UO sere See See Eee ee ee as
22a enl ale Sucka year litte, Seve eee ee ee eee A)
PAGE
2os NEYO) Ol Sea INCOME SME OC cea 48
24. Graph of populations on San Nicolas Island —-____ 51
25. Graph showing changes in populations _____--_______ 53
26. Graph showing changes in amount of suckling —____ 54
x1
27 7 oe oo
Pee
1 7 m
i) ee a 7
- 41
a in Y r
a j eh
: _ a
i L rt "
mal i”
f v a ; fe
fide an dom 2 ee
eet efit et vedi
* mn is
i ?.
Lenaq- Tak Aire wlomnet ’
re Ln deol peter Saha ey aE a
i 1 ‘iy al
bn
7
‘
fy
wey
ul j a
Kny (ui yiveate
INTRODUCTION
HE California sea lion, Zalo-
aL phus californianus (Lesson)
1828, is familiar as a captive ani-
mal, but its behavior under nat-
ural conditions has been little
studied. This is surprising, since <<
it is a species which offers un-
usually favorable opportunities for ethological study; it is highly
gregarious, relatively sedentary on land, conspicuously sexually
dimorphic, locally abundant along the coasts of California and
Baja California, and relatively indifferent to the presence of man.
The latter characteristic is especially important since it minimizes
the effect of a human observer on the social system he is attempt-
ing to study.
As Marler (1963:1081) has pointed out, the social behavior of
pinnipeds is better known than that of any other wild mammals.
Most of this knowledge, however, is based on three genera—the fur
seals (Callorhinus and Arctocephalus) and the elephant seals
(Mirounga). Comparative data from other pinnipeds are sorely
needed, because the behavior of each of the various species is so
specialized that an order-wide analysis of adequate perspective has
never been possible (cf. Bertram, 1940). This paper undertakes to
provide such information on Zalophus, and to make some prelim-
inary analyses relating its behavior to that of other pinnipeds and
to mammals of other orders.
Only fragmentary data are available on the natural history of
Zalophus. Scammon (1874) gave a vivid sealer’s-eye account of its
status during the 19th century. Rowley (1929) presented a general
account of Zalophus behavior (mixed with information on Eume-
topias) , but the value of his report is reduced by his undocumented
assumption that the social organization of sea lions and of fur seals
(Callorhinus) is essentially identical.
The most extensive study of Zalophus behavior is that of Eibl-
Eibesfeldt (1955) who spent 60 hours observing the Galapagos sea
lion (Z. c. wollebaeki). He described a number of unusual behavior
]
2 The California Sea Lion
patterns which have not been reported in other pinnipeds: paternal
care of the young (op. cit.:291); bulls’ pacification of fighting fe-
males (op. cit.:292) ; and territorial activity of pups (op. cit.:294),
for example.
The behavior of Zalophus in captivity has recently interested
several investigators (Schusterman and Feinstein, 1965; Evans and
Haugen, 1963), but their experimental studies have not yet been
extended into the field.
Terminology
An extensive and picturesque terminology developed for pinni-
peds over the centuries of commercial hunting, and some words
have become well established in scientific literature (North Pacific
Fur Seal Commission, 1963). Several of the more useful of these
terms are employed in this study. A bull is an adult male, and a
territorial bull is one defending an area against the intrusion of
other bulls. Pups are young of the year, especially prior to the
autumn molt. A rookery is a coastal area occupied by a breeding
population, and a hauling ground is a terrestrial site occupied by
non-breeding animals.
Methods
The present paper has grown out of a series of studies of the
population dynamics, distribution, and behavior of pinnipeds on
the islands in the Eastern Pacific during the past two decades by
one of us (G.A.B.; see Literature Cited). However, most of the
data on the reproductive biology of Zalophus were obtained during
three and a half months of intensive work on San Nicolas Island,
California, during the late spring and summer of 1965. San Nicolas,
the outermost of the California Channel Islands, is about 66 nautical
miles southwest of Los Angeles Harbor. (For information about
San Nicolas and the other islands in this group, see Hillinger, 1958,
and Winslow, 1960).
OxsERVATION.—The primary method of study was observation
from a blind on a bluff overlooking a rookery on the south shore of
San Nicolas Island (Fig. 1). The behavior of individual sea lions
and their social interactions were recorded in detail in a notebook
at the time they were observed. Characteristic activities were re-
Introduction 3
corded with both still and motion picture photography and vocaliza-
tions were recorded on a Uher 400 Report-S tape recorder. Censuses,
including absolute numbers of all classes of individuals and the lo-
cation and activity of marked individuals, were made on the study
rookery nearly every day between 4 June and 16 September 1965.
The other sea lion rookeries on the island were visited every few
days. Observations were scheduled to sample all hours, day and
night. During the hours of daylight we watched the animals through
binoculars and a spotting telescope so that we were able to keep a
large area, not only that immediately adjacent to our blind, under
surveillance. Our first nocturnal observations were made with an
infra-red “‘sniperscope,’ but we soon found that it was possible to
crawl in among the animals at night and watch them at close range
by available light. This latter method was much more instructive.
We recorded nocturnal observations on a portable tape recorder,
speaking quietly to avoid disturbing the animals.
MarkING.—Thirty-nine pups were marked by clipping out letters
and numbers in the hair of their backs. Pups do not retreat into the
sea when approached, and it was a relatively simple matter to hold
them down and clip the patterns in their hair with a pair of scissors
(Fig. 2). The clipped areas were paler than the rest of the pelage
and the markings persisted for about three months. Eleven adult fe-
males were marked with a black aniline dye (Nyanzol D, Nyanza
Chemical Co.) dissolved in hydrogen peroxide. The dye solution
was placed in a projectile syringe modified by removing the needle
and drilling holes around the distal end. The proximal end, con-
taining a plunger and small explosive charge, was fitted to the head
of an arrow (see Short and King, 1964). The arrows were then
shot at selected female sea lions with a bow, and upon impact the
dye sprayed out and made dark, irregularly shaped blotches which
were still recognizable on the light brown pelage of the females after
more than three months (Fig. 3). When struck by the arrow the
females usually did not flee, but merely reared up and peered about
as if trying to locate the source of the disturbance. This method
allowed us to select specific individuals and mark them without
disturbing the other animals in the rookery.
Zalophus bulls are dark brown, and the black dye proved un-
satisfactory for marking them, particularly when they were wet.
However, most of the territorial bulls bore conspicuous identifying
4 The California Sea Lion
scars, and we were able to identify and follow 14 of them at the
study area by their patterns of scars and wounds.
Experiments in immobilizing bulls with succinylcholine chloride
were undertaken as a possible aid in marking. The drug was in-
jected through an automatic projectile syringe fastened to an
arrow as described above (Fig. 4). Seven bulls were experimented
on in areas remote from the study area. The paralytic dose varied
from 70 to 90 milligrams. The estimated weights of these bulls
ranged from 175 to 275 kg, thus, the dosage was approximately 0.25
to 0.50 mg/kg, very similar to that required for immobilizing Callo-
rhinus bulls (Peterson, 1965a:692) .
General Biology
Zalophus is a medium-sized otariid of marked sexual dimorphism.
Adult males weigh 200-300 kg and are 200-250 cm in length, while
adult females weigh 50-100 kg and measure 150-200 cm. Thus,
adult Zalophus fall in the general size range of Callorhinus (Scheffer,
1958:141—142), but the difference between the male and the female
is somewhat less extreme in Zalophus than in Callorhinus.
Three geographically separated populations of Z. californianus
are recognized by Scheffer (1958) as subspecies: Z. c. wollebaeki in
the Galapagos Archipelago, Z. c. japonicus in Japanese waters, and
Z. c. californianus on the west coast of North America (see also King,
1961:218). The morphologic differences between the three popula-
tions are relatively minor.
Z. c. californianus, on which the present study is based, breeds on
islands in the Gulf of California and along the west coast of Mexico
south to Mazatlan, and off the Baja California and California
coasts northward to San Miguel, the most northwesterly of the
California Channel Islands. Non-breeding aggregations of the spe-
cies occur regularly over a much wider area of the eastern Pacific,
ranging as far north as British Columbia.
A conspicuous, although not complete, sexual segregation occurs
in Z. c. californianus during the non-breeding season. The adult
and sub-adult males generally move northward as soon as the sum-
mer breeding season ends, and return to the rookeries in spring.
The females and young either remain in the vicinity of the breeding
rookeries the year round, or, as in the case of some members of the
Introduction 5
population in the California Channel Islands, apparently move
southward in winter. As a result of the differing seasonal move-
ments of males and females, during the winter there are many fe-
males but few males in the southern half of the range of the sub-
species while many males occur north of the present breeding
range (Fry, 1939; Bartholomew and Hubbs, 1952; Bartholomew and
Boolootian, 1960; Orr and Poulter, 1965). The seasonal segrega-
tion and migration in Zalophus, however, are not nearly so well
defined as that of Callorhinus (Kenyon and Wilke, 1953).
The food of Zalophus consists primarily of squid, including Lo-
ligo, and small fish such as Engraulis and Merluccius (Bonnot,
1928; Scheffer and Neff, 1948; Fiscus and Baines, 1966). From ship-
board we have seen Zalophus that appeared to be feeding during
both day and night, but it is not known if they have a daily feeding
cycle as do Callorhinus (Fiscus et al., 1964:36) and Leptonychotes
(M. S. R. Smith, 1965:15).
NON-REPRODUCTIVE BEHAVIOR
Locomotion
N WateER.—The swimming of Zalophus re-
l sembles that of other otariids (see Howell,
1930, and Ray, 1963). The main propulsive
force is supplied by simultaneous inward and
downward strokes of the foreflippers, while
the hindflippers function primarily as stabi-
lizers or as rudders, depending upon their plane
of rotation. When turning horizontally, sea
lions extend their foreflippers and bank like a turning bird.
Contrary to statements of Rowley (1929:19), sea lions frequently
“porpoise” while swimming; that is, they leap from the water in a
shallow arc and re-enter head first. This pattern of behavior 1s
most common during sustained, rapid swimming. Groups of 5 to 20
young individuals sometimes swim in single file, porpoising one
after another. An intruder being chased at top speed by a territorial
bull usually porpoises while fleeing from its attacker. Young animals
often leap exuberantly out of the water during play and may
employ porpoising in play chases.
Zalophus use waves to assist them in coming ashore and going
to sea. They are skillful “body-surfers’” and often glide down the
front of a wave, just submerged or partly above the surface. After
the wave breaks they ride it up to the beach, keeping their heads
and sometimes shoulders above the foam. Animals coming ashore
on a level beach usually take advantage of incoming waves, and
they ride the backwash when departing. On a steep, rocky shore,
they often utilize a wave to assist in vertical movement and may
leap from the top of the wave to land on a ledge which could other-
wise be reached only after much climbing.
Ageregations of sea lions commonly sleep in the water, often in
protected areas near the shore. Sleeping animals frequently lie on
one side and elevate a foreflipper or hindflipper above the water
as described by Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1955:299) , but we have never seen
them assume the “jug” posture of fur seals, that is, floating on one
6
Non-reproductive Behavior 7
side with the hindflippers and one foreflipper pressed together and
held in an arch above the water. Zalophus is essentially a coastal
animal that hauls out on shore frequently throughout the year, and
as such may not need to be so well adapted to sleeping in the water
as a pinniped like Callorhinus which remains at sea for months at
a time. Furthermore, Zalophus may be less bouyant than the fur
seals (which have thicker pelage) ; C. H. Fiscus, who has studied
seals in the pelagic environment, reports (pers. comm.) that both
Eumetopias and Zalophus are much more difficult to collect at sea
than fur seals because the sea lions usually sink when they are shot.
On Lanp.—Zalophus, like other otariids, but unlike phocids, are
capable of quite effective locomotion on land. In difficult terrain,
such as among slippery rocks, an aggressive bull can probably move
more rapidly than a man. Top speed across smooth rocks probably
approaches 15 miles per hour, but this can be maintained for a few
meters only.
Terrestrial locomotion is extremely variable, but several basic
patterns can be discerned. ‘These may be used simultaneously and
in various combinations. The points of traction are the whole
manus of the foreflipper and posterior portion (heel) of the hind-
flipper. When moving slowly, the animals walk, using all four
limbs alternately, much like a terrestrial quadruped. However, the
hind limbs are incapable of moving independently more than a
short distance and so the steps are quite short. As the animal walks,
the fore and hind limbs are swung laterally as well as forward, and
barely clear the ground. With each step the shoulders and hips are
rotated strongly, and the head and neck swing sinuously from side
to side (Fig. 5). Sometimes, when moving in a leisurely manner, a
sea lion will flex the posterior half of its vertebral column laterally
with each step so that the hindquarters are displaced to one side
with every stride. Some individuals walking in this manner seem
always to swing the hindquarters to the left, others always to the
right; in a group of non-territorial bulls, we once counted 16 swing-
ing to the left and 11 to the right.
When moving more rapidly, sea lions gallop, using the heels of
both hindflippers as a single pivot while the foreflippers move
forward slightly out of phase with each other. A galloping animal’s
head does not swing laterally, but bobs up and down vertically.
8 The California Sea Lion
Galloping pups move their foreflippers simultaneously in a rapid,
rotary motion.
While moving over smooth sand, going downhill, or in a few
inches of water, Zalophus often stride with long steps of the fore-
limbs, and drag the hindquarters limply along behind, sometimes
with the hindflippers elevated slightly so that the weight rests on
the abdomen. This striding pattern permits very rapid movement
across wet flats or in the wash of the surf, and frequently is em-
ployed by territorial males when rushing at intruders.
An intruding bull or a submissive, estrous female will sometimes
crawl along slowly on its belly by alternate lateral movements of
its flippers, keeping the head and neck close to the ground. This
crawling is somewhat like the “sinuous” locomotion of Lobodon car-
cinophagus described by O’Gorman (1963:847) .
Individual Mannerisms on Land
SLEEPING PosTuRES.—Sea lions have extremely flexible bodies and
assume a wide variety of resting and sleeping postures while on land.
Several of these are quite stereotyped and are regularly employed
by individuals of both sexes and all ages. In one characteristic
sleeping posture the animal lies prostrate on its belly and folds all
four flippers underneath. In another, the animal lies on its side with
the foreflippers against the body and the hindflippers appressed
and extended to the rear. Zalophus also sleep sitting up on the fore-
flippers, with the head thrown back and the nose pointing vertically,
evidently balancing themselves comfortably in this way (Figs. 6
and 7).
GrooMING.—Despite their short sparse hair (Scheffer, 1964:293) ,
Zalophus spend much time grooming, just as do marine mammals
such as Callorhinus and Enhydra, which have long dense pelage.
The most conspicuous and frequent grooming mannerism of Zalo-
phus is a dog-like scratching with one of the hindflippers. By bend-
ing the body laterally, a sea lion can groom nearly any part of the
anterior two-thirds of the body by this means. The digits of the
flipper being used are flexed so that the terminal flaps fold down
and the three functional claws are exposed (Fig. 8). Another com-
mon pattern of grooming involves rubbing with one foreflipper
while balancing on the other; the head and neck are usually
Fic. 1. Study area on southwest shore of San Nicolas Island. See Fig. 23 for
precise location.
es
user
Ce So is
Fic. 2. Pup that has been marked by hair clipping; grooming itself by scratch-
ing dog-fashion with hindflipper (top) .
Fic. 3. Female with dye-mark on flank (bottom) .
Non-reproductive Behavior 9
stretched upward and the palmar surface of the manus, particularly
near its leading edge, is rubbed back and forth across the body.
Almost the entire posterior half of the body can be reached this way.
The foreflipper can also be rotated forward and used to rub the
head when the neck is bent laterally. Less frequently, Zalophus
rub their snouts across their hindquarters, and nuzzle or nibble
their pelage with their incisors as dogs do.
Individuals of both sexes and all ages rub their bodies against
rocks or against other animals, similar to ungulates at a rubbing post.
‘They may also lie flat on the sand or on a smooth rock and rub
their bellies or backs with lateral flexures of the body. Another
characteristic grooming mannerism consists of rubbing the vibrissae
against a smooth rock with a firm stropping motion, first on one
side of the snout and then the other. Such “whisker-stropping”’ is
frequently repeated by adults just after they have hauled out of
the water.
ExcretTion.—Unlike most terrestrial mammals, Zalophus assume
no special postures when urinating or defecating, nor do they pay
any attention to voided wastes. They may defecate or urinate when
lying prostrate or while moving about on the rookery. The fre-
quency of defecation is good evidence that both the territorial bulls
and nursing females feed regularly during the course of the breed-
ing season.
Zalophus, particularly nursing females, sometimes vomit a white
milky fluid and emit a characteristic coughing noise in the process.
This regurgitation is accompanied by violent contractions of the
thorax and by lateral shaking of the head; it appears to be labored
and difficult.
Drinkinc.—Although field observations on the drinking of sea
water are always hard to verify, we saw several instances that warrant
description. Territorial bulls immersed their snouts in pools of
sea water and performed gulping motions, interspersed with chew-
ing movements of the jaws after the head was lifted from the water.
We do not know if water was ingested, or whether this behavior was
merely mouth-wetting. We have both observed similar behavior
among bulls of Callorhinus on warm days.
RESPONSES TO WEATHER.—Although Zalophus breed successfully
in the tropics (in the Galapagos Islands, for example) , individuals
appear to be sensitive to high air temperatures and direct solar
10 The California Sea Lion
radiation. On San Nicolas Island the sky is covered with a low,
unbroken overcast at least 25 per cent of the time during the
summer (Winslow, 1960:30). Whenever the sky clears and direct
sunlight strikes the rookeries, those sea lions not already at the
water’s edge arouse and move onto wet sand or into shallow water.
Bulls appear to be especially sensitive, and during sunny days most
of them keep thoroughly wet by frequent visits to the water.
Females also go to the water’s edge frequently during the day.
The pups, however, are much less responsive to heat. A female
that is still defensive of her new pup may drag it with her when
she visits the edge of the sea or a tide pool, probably because she
needs to cool off and is unwilling to be separated from her pup,
not because the pup needs protection from heat.
On bright, clear days, sea lions sleeping in a prostrate position
often extend their hind- and foreflippers upward and hold them
motionless. By analogy with the more elaborate flipper-waving
of Callorhinus (Bartholomew and Wilke, 1956:331, and Irving
et al., 1962:282), this behavior could serve to dissipate heat. But
in Zalophus, these postures are sometimes seen on cool, overcast days,
and pups also use them. Thus, we cannot be certain that a thermo-
regulatory function is served. The habit of staying damp or resting
on damp sand whenever heat stress exists may be sufficient in Zalo-
phus to unload excessive heat. It may be significant that we have
never seen behavior that we can classify with certainty as panting,
even by bulls engaged in vigorous fighting.
Paulian (1964:28) suggests that the distribution of Arctocephalus
on the windward side of Ile Amsterdam may be related to this fur
seal’s need for cooling. It is possible that the distribution of Zalo-
phus on the Channel Islands, including San Nicolas, may be simi-
larly related to wind direction. The great majority of the strong,
steady winds on these islands are from the west and northwest
(Winslow, 1960:29), and almost all the rookeries are on beaches
with western exposures.
Strong winds have little effect on Zalophus, but if the surf is
heavy the animals move inland above the breakers. Stinging sand
storms occur frequently on the windward side of San Nicolas, but
the animals seem indifferent to the pelting sand and allow it to
drift around their bodies.
Non-reproductive Behavior 11
Special Senses
Viston.—The visual acuity of Zalophus in air
is apparently limited. Even in broad daylight we
were able to approach within two meters or less
of aggregations of females and pups so long as we
crawled slowly and kept our silhouettes low. How-
ever, a man in full view walking toward sea lions
will alarm them at 75 meters or more during the
day, and at 25 meters on a clear night.
When a strange object is near them (a crawling
man, for example) sea lions extend their necks toward it and peer
intently, swinging their lowered heads from side to side as if trying
to see it better. Once when we were lying prostrate on the open
beach at midday recording sea lion vocalizations, a female ap-
proached the canvas-covered tape recorder which was lying beside
one of us, peered at it for about ten seconds, sniffed it, and then
settled to sleep about a meter away. When the recorder clicked
softly as we threw a switch, however, the animal bolted away. The
sight of a man with a parka over his head, a microphone in his
hand, and a recorder beside him had not been enough to cause
alarm, but a simple clicking sound caused a strong flight reaction.
We have had full-grown bulls actually brush against us in broad
daylight while we were sitting fully exposed but with our outlines
obscured by a boulder against which we were leaning.
As discussed later, Zalophus are as active at night as during the
day, but their visual discrimination at night is even poorer than
during the day. At night, by remaining close to a cliff so that we
were not outlined against the sky, we were able to crawl into the
aggregations of females and pups and remain for as long as we
wished, so long as we did not allow the animals to sniff our faces.
However, despite their apparently poor aerial vision, sea lions
readily discriminate bold outlines and rapid movements. Any
bulky vertical object rising above the general level of the females’
heads can cause an alarm response, especially if it moves. A vertical
image of this sort is presented by a threatening territorial bull
standing with elevated head and neck, as well as by a man standing
up. We frequently saw aggregations of sea lions take alarm at a
threatening, silhouetted bull, exactly as they would at a man ap-
proaching on foot.
f
’
j 9)
12 The California Sea Lion
The visual signals utilized by sea lions in social interactions are
simple and obvious and require little more than perception of size
and movement. An example is the boundary ceremony of territorial
bulls, in which the head is held high and swung slowly from side
to side.
Hamilton (1934:294) has described behavior in Otaria indicative
of limited visual acuity and he suggests without further documenta-
tion that this may be related to hyper-sensitivity in bright light
or to irritation of the eyes by constant wind. On the basis of ocular
morphology it has been suggested that the eyes of pinnipeds are
adapted for acute vision in air as well as in water (Walls, 1942:446) .
Although Zalophus may employ relatively precise visual discrimina-
tion when feeding under water (Schusterman et al., 1965; Hobson,
1966) , it appears that their visual discrimination in air is of a low
order. Judging by their reactions to man, some pinnipeds such as
harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) see much better in air than do sea
lions.
Hearinc.—The California sea lion is among the most vocal of
mammals; a wide variety of vocal signals are continuously being
exchanged between adults. As discussed later, the mutual recogni-
tion of mother and pup appear to depend primarily upon identifi-
cation of individual vocalizations. These facts are circumstantial
evidence for good hearing ability, yet Zalophus are relatively un-
responsive to sounds such as those of aircraft or even the low conver-
sation of humans only one to two meters distant. Sharp, sudden
sounds cause immediate alarm, however, and every animal in a
noisy aggregation will rise up in alarm if two rocks are struck to-
gether. ‘They seem habituated to loud, general noises such as that
of hundreds of surrounding animals and the crashing of the surf,
while simultaneously discriminating and reacting to particular
vocalizations of nearby individuals.
Zalophus may possess the ability to echolocate underwater (see
Evans and Haugen, 1963; Poulter, 1963; Schusterman and Fein-
stein, 1965); this would be additional reason to suspect an acute
auditory sense. From the importance of vocal signaling in their
behavior on land, it seems certain that the sense is highly developed.
CHEMICAL SENsES.—Olfactory clues apparently are employed by
Zalophus in at least two social situations on land: the interactions
between males and estrous females, and in mother-pup recognition.
Non-reproductive Behavior 13
In the latter situation, at least, a high level of olfactory discrimina-
tion appears to be involved.
Bulls of Zalophus have a slight musky odor, but it is not nearly as
strong as that of Callorhinus. We saw nothing to suggest that
bulls of Zalophus use scent to mark their territories as has been
postulated for Callorhinus (Peterson, 1965b:81). However, the
rocks and sand of the rookeries are extremely odoriferous, as we
found when crawling among the animals. It is not difficult to imag-
ine that these smells, mostly resulting from urine and feces, could be
used to recognize established hauling-out places, even when there
were no other animals on shore.
Several instances of the apparent use of olfaction occurred after
the carcass of a dead pup fell into a small, stagnant pool where
adults had been wetting themselves frequently on warm days. On
9 June 1965, we watched a succession of bulls and females approach
this pool as if about to immerse themselves in it, only to move away
after sniffing at the floating carcass. ,
Like Otaria (Hamilton, 1934:295) and Callorhinus (authors’
observations) , Zalophus may be able at times to detect the smell of
humans. On 11 June 1965, in the company of four seamen who
were to assist us in marking, we approached the study area from
upwind and remained completely out of sight of the sea lions. Never-
theless the entire rookery took alarm while we were more than 100
yards distant, out of sight behind a hill. It is possible that the ani-
mals detected the odor of our sweating bodies, yet during our ex-
cursions among the animals at night individual sea lions seldom
took alarm until they sniffed directly and carefully at our faces.
It may be that these animals are capable of discriminating various
odors, and react selectively to especially strange or strong ones.
We have not seen any behavior which suggests that sea lions use
their gustatory sense while on land. During hundreds of hours of
observation we have never seen female Zalophus lick their newborn
pups (contra Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1955:300) and it appears improbable
that taste functions in mother-young recognition in the population
we studied. King (1964:124) reported that the tongue of Zalophus
has a poor supply of taste buds.
TACTILE SENSE.—The frequency of social nuzzling and nose-rubbing
in Zalophus suggests that the long vibrissae are highly developed
sensors (Fig. 10). Vibrissal touching occurs in almost every close
14 The California Sea Lion
interaction between pairs or groups of sea lions: between females
and pups, bulls and females, pairs of threatening bulls, pairs of
threatening females, and among young non-breeders. The vibris-
sae are extremely movable and can be rotated from the folded-
back resting position until they extend almost directly forward
from the muzzle. We found that when curious sea lions sniffed
at our faces at night, they extended their vibrissae forward and
gently brushed them against us, while repeatedly opening their
nostrils.
Except in the breeding season, Zalophus are strongly thigmotactic,
and usually lie in closely packed groups, occasionally even on top
of each other. At times, however, they become quite reactive to any
light touch; females with newborn pups, for example, react very
aggressively to even the lightest contact from another adult or pup.
Thus, it appears that the skin of Zalophus may be quite sensitive to
touch (as is that of Monachus, Kenyon and Rice, 1959:234), but
that responses to tactile stimuli, as with those to visual, acoustic,
and olfactory stimuli, are selective.
Interspecific Social Relations
The rookeries and hauling grounds of Zalophus are regularly
visited by scavengers and are also utilized by other marine verte-
brates for breeding and resting. Consequently, there are frequent
opportunities for observing the relations between Zalophus and
other species. ‘The other pinnipeds which commonly occur in the
California Channel Islands, Mirouwnga angustirostris, Phoca vitulina,
and Eumetopias jubata, frequently share beaches with Zalophus,
but no two of the species breed during the same season at the same
location. Brandt’s cormorants (Phalacrocorax penicillatus) some-
times breed on the same beaches with Zalophus, and western gulls
(Larus occidentalis) breed immediately inland of the sea lion rook-
eries. The western gulls, as well as ravens (Corvus corax), and
Channel Island foxes (Urocyon littoralis) scavenge among the sea
lions.
ELEPHANT SEALS.—On San Nicolas Island only a few elephant
seals come ashore during the sea lions’ breeding season. The ex-
tensive and sustained interactions of Mirounga and Zalophus have
been described elsewhere (Bartholomew, 1952:380-382), and we
Non-reproductive Behavior 15
have little to add here. In general, the interspecific relations of
these two species resemble the intraspecific behavior of sea lions
under similar circumstances; that is, there is a weak dominance
relationship among the non-breeders based upon individual size,
and strong territoriality among breeding bulls and parturient fe-
males. Territorial bulls of Zalophus threaten and chase elephant
seals that come too close, just as they would any other large animal
that approached, and even female Zalophus may react aggressively
toward small elephant seals. On 5 June 1965, we watched a female
Zalophus, still vigorously defending her newborn pup, threaten a
young elephant seal that was moving toward her. The elephant seal
reared up briefly as the female approached, then changed its course
and moved aside.
Harsor SEAts.—Aggregations of Phoca frequently haul out on
active Zalophus rookeries, but harbor seals tend to remain in com-
pact groups and do not intermingle freely with the sea lions. Sea
lion bulls sometimes threaten individual harbor seals in the water
near shore, and chase them briefly. On land the harbor seals are
treated with almost complete indifference, as though they were
sea lion pups or submissive juveniles. On 20 June 1965, territorial
bull Zalophus were defending territories on San Nicolas area 3B
where throughout the summer aggregations of harbor seals persisted.
Whenever one of the bulls approached, the aggregations of Phoca
would quickly part to let him pass through, much as a pod of sea
lion pups would do. Individual sea lions and harbor seals usually
avoid contact with each other; we never saw Zalophus crawling
over Phoca as they do across each other or across Mirounga. But
since harbor seals move more slowly than sea lions on land, individ-
uals of the two genera sometimes meet. When this happens, the
harbor seal usually rolls on its side, keeps its head lifted upright,
snaps its jaws at the approaching sea lion, and simultaneously makes
a rapid waving or scratching motion in the air with the uppermost
foreflipper. We never saw a harbor seal actually bite or scratch a
sea lion, but on one occasion we saw a female Zalophus with a new
pup viciously chase a harbor seal that had landed quietly nearby.
She dashed toward it and made a few jabbing nips at its back; the
harbor seal quickly abandoned its threatening posture and scurried
rapidly away.
16 The California Sea Lion
Harbor seals are readily alarmed and appear to have much more
acute vision than sea lions. It is, therefore, difficult for a man to
approach a mixed aggregation of sea lions and harbor seals, since
the harbor seals usually see him and dash for the water, and then
the sea lions react by joining the stampede. The converse does not
hold, for harbor seals do not react to the frequent, self-induced
panics of young sea lions playing among them. When the sea lions
make such a rush toward the water, the harbor seals raise their
heads and look about alertly, but settle to sleep again if no danger
is sighted.
STELLER SEA Lions.—The breeding ranges of Zalophus and Eume-
topias overlap at the present time only on San Miguel Island (Bar-
tholomew and Boolootian, 1960:373), and the breeding season of
Eumetopias occurs before that of Zalophus. Mixed non-breeding
ageregations of the two genera are common north of the breeding
range of Zalophus and the interactions in these groups have been
described by Orr (1965a:165). The interspecific relations are gen-
erally parallel to those which occur between Mirounga and Zalo-
phus, in that the animals tend to interact in the usual intraspecific
manner.
GuLtis.—Western gulls were continuously present on the sea lion
rookeries, even at night. Breeding colonies of the gulls occurred
within 50 meters or less of the sea lion rookeries at San Nicolas Area
1. The gulls are very bold and walk about among the sea lions
quite freely, sometimes even alighting on them. They are particu-
larly responsive to fresh placentas, and each birth attracts a noisy
flock of gulls almost immediately. The gulls also eat Zalophus feces.
They sometimes peck at the anus of a pup, following it about the
rookery in their attempts.
For the most part, sea lions pay no attention to the scavenging
gulls, but occasionally a female reacts to a nearby gull with an
open-mouth threat. Pups and juvenile animals sometimes snap at
the gulls and chase them briefly. Very young pups occasionally
appear to be frightened of the gulls.
Sea lions are sensitive to sudden movements and sharp noises.
Thus, any excitement among the gulls, or their alarm calls, may
cause a widespread reaction among the sea lions. A single alarm
call from a gull, for example, can cause one sea lion to dash toward
the water, and this movement may set off a general stampede that
ne
Fic. 4. Bow-and-arrow technique used for marking and narcotizing animals
(top). See text for details.
Fic. 5. Female and pup moving from one territory to another (bottom).
Note strong lateral movement of head and neck as she walks. Pup in foreground
has responded to vocalizations that female directed at her own pup and is mov-
ing toward the pair.
Fic. 6. Prostrate sleeping postures of pups (top) .
Fic. 7. Female sleeping in characteristic vertical posture in a semi-aquatic
territory (bottom). Note territorial male in water to right of females.
Non-reproductive Behavior 17
will empty the beach of animals. Sometimes the birds may hover
and squawk near an aggregation where there is a fresh placenta,
almost as though intentionally trying to frighten the sea lions away.
But the sea lions rarely react strongly to this situation. ‘They ap-
pear to be relatively unresponsive to disturbances in the breeding
rookeries during the breeding season.
Man.—Although one can closely approach sea lions with only the
most rudimentary sort of stalking, this is apparently owing to the
sea lions’ poor aerial vision rather than indifference to the presence
of man. On the California Channel Islands, the usual reaction of
sea lions as soon as they become aware of the approach of humans
is a wild stampede into the water. This reaction is not invariable
in the species, however. On the Galapagos Islands, individuals of
the subspecies Z. c. wollebaeki share the indifference to man which
is so conspicuous of most vertebrates of oceanic islands. At Punta
Espinosa on Narborough Island in the Galapagos, some of the
Zalophus were so unafraid of man that one of us (G.A.B.) was able
to walk directly up to and touch adult females, and on one occasion
was able to play tug-of-war for several minutes with a juvenile male
which held a piece of driftwood in its jaws like a dog with a stick
(see also Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1955:298) .
During the breeding season, the alarm reaction of the Zalophus
in the Channel Islands population changes markedly. The with-
drawal of an aggregation at the approach of a man becomes rela-
tively slow, almost reluctant. In June and July, a territorial bull
may refuse to retreat and may chase a man to its territorial bound-
ary (Fig. 9). On 3 July 1965, we found one bull alone on a beach
with the carcass of a female; this bull reacted aggressively when we
approached, and refused to retreat (Fig. 10).
Similarly, females with new pups are not apt to flee from a man
and will remain on the beach with their pups long after the other
adults have dashed into the water. Occasionally, they even rush
threateningly at a man.
Chronic human disturbance causes Zalophus to abandon a rook-
ery temporarily. Our repeated efforts to mark some animals at the
beginning of the study caused many females and pups to move, over
a period of two days, to a new site 30 to 50 meters away. Over a
period of several weeks they gradually drifted back and reoccupied
the old site. If rookery disturbance persists for years, as has hap-
18 The California Sea Lion
pened at part of San Nicolas Area 1 because of military operations,
the site is finally abandoned entirely.
Zalophus are much less fearful of man in the water than on land,
and they often approach swimmers and small boats closely. Groups
of 15-30 young individuals (up to 3 years old) frequently rush to
meet a boat and may accompany it for 100 meters or more showing
every sign of excited curiosity.
Non-reproductive Social Interactions
During the non-breeding season (ap-
proximately August through April), the
aggregations of sea lions on land have no
stable social organization. ‘The same is
also true on the hauling grounds (as con-
trasted with the rookeries) throughout
the breeding season. However, certain
stereotyped interactions can be seen in these unorganized groups,
and the behavior follows the general patterns described in this
section.
DoMINANCE RELATIONS.—There is a clear, size-related dominance
relationship among non-breeders, regardless of sex. When a small
individual is approached or threatened by a larger one, the smaller
usually yields by scrambling aside or running, but there is no sus-
tained aggressive behavior during these encounters. The hierarchi-
cal relations are weak and transient, but readily apparent in any
non-breeding aggregation.
GREGARIOUSNESS.—Non-breeding individuals are always highly gre-
garious while on land. They often pack themselves so closely to-
gether that arriving or departing animals must crawl over the bodies
OW AWA =~ j
of the others. ‘These congested groups form even though large sec-
tions of adjacent beach remain unoccupied. The animals are
strongly thigmotactic and while sleeping tend to keep their bodies
pressed against each other. Even non-territorial bulls lie close to-
gether in this way, especially in winter. Sometimes small individ-
uals may lie on top of larger ones, but this is not tolerated for ex-
tended periods of time. They also rest on the backs of elephant
seals or on piles of kelp, seemingly preferring these soft resting
places to rocks.
Non-reproductive Behavior 19
PLay.—It is a common observation that sea lions spend much time
and energy at play, both on land and at sea (see, for example,
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1955:297). Play is difficult behavior to categorize
and describe (Loizos, 1966). In sea lions, as in other mammals,
play is most frequent among non-breeders, especially juveniles.
The activity of pups will be described separately.
The most common pattern of play involves swimming about in
the surf. An animal may ride down the front of a wave, usually
just beneath the surface, until it breaks, then veer sharply to one
side inside the wave, or even leap out over the crest, and then swim
back out to await another wave. Sometimes several animals ride
one wave together, and they may tumble over each other in the
breakers. Frequently they chase each other in the water, porpois-
ing and turning sharply. Often three or four juveniles engage
in play contests over occupancy of a boulder or ledge in shallow
water. One animal will climb onto the boulder and try to remain
there as the others climb out of the water and try to displace it by
shoving and pushing. Such contests may continue for a quarter of
an hour.
Subadult males (probably sexually mature but not yet full
grown) sometimes threaten each other almost as though they were
on territories, using the same postures and movements as the terri-
torial bulls. Non-breeding bulls also behave in this manner. They
chase after each other, bark continually, stand chest-to-chest, push
each other, shake their heads from side to side, and exchange open-
mouthed threats, all of which appear to be intermediate between
the play of young pups and the more aggressive interactions of terri-
torial bulls. Wounds are not inflicted, and groups, rather than
pairs, frequently participate in the skirmishes. ‘Territorial bound-
aries are not maintained. For discussion of this behavior see sec-
tion on ontogeny of aggressive behavior.
ALARM REAcTIONS.—On the California Channel Islands, the usual
alarm reaction of non-breeding Zalophus consists of a frantic dash
for the water, even if this involves leaping off a high cliff onto a
rocky beach below. The alarm is highly contagious, and usually
spreads rapidly through an aggregation. If the reaction is of low
intensity several animals will simultaneously rear up and look
about and then gradually retreat from the direction of the disturb-
ance, or if the disturbance is more obvious they will rush to the
20 The California Sea Lion
water’s edge before stopping to look about. Sometimes hundreds
of animals stampede into the water without pausing to identify the
source of their disturbance. After they enter the water, they typi-
cally begin barking, form groups (rafts) , and swim back toward the
shore with heads held high from the water as though trying to ob-
serve the cause of their alarm.
In our experience, the immediate causes of these stampedes were
human activity, alarm calls or other sudden activity of gulls, escape
reactions of cormorants or harbor seals, a loud noise such as the
slap of a flipper on a wet rock, or indeed any sudden movement,
even that of another sea lion. Sometimes the sight of a bull stand-
ing at its full height near the inland edge of a rookery causes alarm,
and on warm and sunny days the stampedes may occur for no ap-
parent reason.
This alarm reaction is probably learned by the pups during their
first few weeks of life. During their first week or so, despite their
precociousness, they show no fear of man and do not join in stam-
pedes. But by the time they are about a month old they begin to
react like the adults. We have already noted that adult Galapagos
sea lions show virtually no fear of man, so it is entirely possible
that the alarm reaction is learned and “‘culturally” transmitted in
the California population.
Fic. 8. Close-up of a pup scratching dog-fashion. Note that the toes are flexed
to expose the claws.
Fic. 9. Territorial bull charging a man (top).
Fic. 10. Bull defending area adjacent to carcass of dead female (bottom) .
Note the long vibrissae and scars on the neck and chest.
REPRODUCTIVE BEHAVIOR
ALIFORNIA sea lions are highly po-
ic lygynous; the ratio of females to
males in our study area during the
1965 breeding season averaged 14: 1.
The typical rookery consists of terri-
torial bulls spaced fairly uniformly in
a single line along a beach, with fe-
males in dense aggregations at irreg-
ular intervals among the bulls. This pattern of spatial organiza-
tion is determined primarily by the strong gregariousness of the
females, and by the extreme aggressiveness of bulls. The arrange-
ment of the territorial bulls depends upon the presence of females
to a large extent, although in some places the bulls defend terri-
tories on stretches of shoreline when no females are present. Most
of the unemployed males gather instead on a few separate beaches,
and on these hauling grounds they form non-territorial aggregations
similar to those that exist during fall and winter.
BREEDING SiTES.—As is the case in most pinnipeds, breeding in
Zalophus tends to be restricted to a small number of the potentially
suitable localities available. Although they tend to breed on the
same sections of the coast year after year, they tend to be less
permanently fixed to traditional sites than is the case in Callorhinus
(Kenyon, 1960:440). For example, during the breeding season an
entire breeding aggregation of Zalophus may shift its location by
hundreds of meters because of repeated disturbance, sustained heavy
surf, or for no discernible reason.
On the Channel Islands, Zalophus usually breed on shores ex-
posed to the prevailing winds, as discussed earlier. Rookeries exist
on both rocky shores and sandy beaches, but the locally preferred
habitat appears to be rocky flats and shelves, especially those which
are occasionally washed by large waves or submerged at high tide.
Such terrain not only keeps the animals wetted, but is frequently
cleaned of carcasses and wastes by action of the sea. At present, of
course, one of the important determinants of rookery location is the
local pattern of distribution of humans.
il
22 The California Sea Lion
Male Territories
SHORELINE Locations.—Almost all territories of Zalophus bulls
extend to the water’s edge, a feature that affects the entire system
of social organization. Access to the sea seems to be required by
bulls as a part of their habit of keeping continually wetted on sunny
days. Bulls that station themselves inland of the line of territories
that abuts on the water are usually transient and often abandon
their territories after only a few hours. Bulls sometimes establish
and maintain temporary landlocked territories at night, only to
abandon them in the morning. Inland bulls often are smaller and
presumably younger than those in the first row along the shore,
and thus may not be sufficiently large or experienced to hold a
stretch of beach in face of competition from other bulls. Occasion-
ally, tide pools are used for wetting, and a few bulls hold inland
territories centered on these.
On San Nicolas Island the vertical tidal excursion is about three
feet, so that the flat, sandy beaches used for rookeries are awash at
high tide. Consequently, many male territories are partly aquatic.
Some bulls even maintain wholly aquatic territories immediately
offshore from the rookeries, staying on station in the water even
when the surf is heavy (Fig. 11).
S1zE OF TERRITORIES.—The territories of Zalophus bulls are poorly
defined. Like those of Arctocephalus (Paulian, 1964:96), they
vary considerably between individuals, depending upon movements
of females, temperature, and time of day. The specific territorial
boundaries are hard to map for several reasons: (1) typically there
is no inland boundary which a bull defends, (2) the seaward edge
of the territory may be underwater, (3) a bull only infrequently
patrols the entire perimeter of his territory, and (4) intruding
bulls are sometimes tolerated in territories by the resident bull for
hours at a time, very probably because he remains unaware of them.
On the spacious, flat beach (Area 6B) east of our observation post,
many bulls were spaced along the shore at 10- to 15-meter intervals
during early June (Fig. 12); this probably represents an idealized
distribution during the breeding season. On the rocky, irregular ter-
rain that constituted our study area, six or eight territories, recog-
nizable but irregular in shape, were usually maintained.
Reproductive Behavior 23
DurRATION OF TERRITORY MAINTENANCE.—We have seen bulls de-
fending territories during May, June, July, and August, but the
territorial behavior is most intense and sustained in late June and
early July, at which time some bulls even become aggressive toward
humans. Although territories are maintained over a period of four
months, individual Zalophus bulls do not stay on land continuously
throughout the breeding season; in fact, few individuals remain
at one location for more than two weeks. As discussed in a later
section, during June and July, 1965, the duration of territory main-
tenance of 5 different bulls which could be individually identified
from scars and marks had a mean of 9 days. Several of these
bulls were seen at other places around San Nicolas Island during
the summer, but at these times they were not holding territories.
Two lines of indirect evidence suggest that few bulls hold terri-
tories much longer than the individuals in our small sample: (1)
the bulls do not become thin and emaciated toward the end of
the breeding season as do many fur seal bulls, and (2) Zalophus
bulls occasionally defecate on land, strongly suggesting that they are
not fasting for prolonged periods.
Although the rate of turnover of bulls in the rookery is high in
comparison with other polygynous pinnipeds that have been stud-
ied, the spacing and distribution of the bulls remains relatively
constant throughout June and July. A new bull does not usually
preempt parts of established territories; instead, it takes over the
same stretch of shoreline that was held by the animal it displaced.
As a result, a given territory may be occupied by a succession of
different bulls during the breeding season. This contrasts with
Callorhinus and Mirounga rookeries, which become progressively
more crowded with bulls during the early breeding season (Peter-
son, 1965b:82; Laws, 1956:77) .
EsTABLISHMENT OF TERRITORIAL STATUS.—We do not know how
the first bulls in a given season establish their territorial status,
but once some bulls are established, subsequent individuals must
fight to gain a place. The motor activities associated with fighting
appear to be aimed primarily at physically forcing the opponent out
of a territory. The commonest pattern consists of chest-to-chest
pushing. Quick, slashing bites and more prolonged grappling ma-
neuvers accompany pushing, but almost always seem to be directed
toward the same goal, physical displacement of the opponent. Fights
24 The California Sea Lion
occur both on land and in the water. The longest fights take place
in shallow water, where the animals have greater maneuverability
than on land, but where it is more difficult for a defeated animal to
retreat than would be the case in water deep enough for easy swim-
ming. Fights in deep water are very vigorous but usually brief. The
defeated individual can readily escape, not only because of the
agility of sea lions in water, but also because of the three dimen-
sional nature of an aquatic territory.
Bulls on established territories bark incessantly. An intruding
male, in contrast, is silent and rapidly approaches the territorial
bull that it is going to attack with every indication of purposeful-
ness but without vocalizing. A fight usually begins as soon as the
intruder crosses the territorial boundary, since the established bull
generally moves rapidly to meet the intruder. The ensuing fight
may last several minutes. It consists of a series of violent struggles,
separated by brief pauses during which the bulls stand chest-to-chest.
During these pauses, the contestants hold their heads high in the
air as if each were trying to tower above the other. Then, as the
pause ends, each bull weaves its head and neck laterally several
times and finally lunges again at the opponent’s chest, flank, or
foreflipper. During these lunges the animals often fall flat on their
chests, necks extended. The massive, well-padded chest and the
neck receive most of the wounds and seem to act as a shield for the
more vulnerable flippers. If the animals are half submerged in the
water, their efforts at seizing each other’s flanks and foreflippers
may lead them into a circular chase, sometimes with each attempt-
ing to seize the hindflippers of the other. Once a bull is bested,
either by being pushed out of the territory in question, or by being
overpowered and apparently losing confidence, he tries to retreat
while at the same time protecting his vulnerable hindquarters. A
retreating bull, therefore, repeatedly whirls to face his pursuing
opponent.
A fight seldom involves more than two bulls. When the fighting
pair blunders into the territory of a third bull, this neighbor
sometimes attempts to bite the flipper of whichever bull he can
reach. Fights involving several bulls at their territorial intersects,
as seen in Callorhinus (Bartholomew and Hoel, 1953:484; Peterson,
1965b:67), were never observed in Zalophus. This probably is a
function of the lower level of aggressiveness, and rather indefinite
ee
Fic. 11. Two bulls maintaining adjacent territories in the water at one edge
of study area.
Fic. 12. Typical spatial arrangement in a rookery on a sandy beach with ani-
mals on damp sand at water’s edge. Territorial bulls are spaced at intervals of
10 to 15 meters, and the confluent female aggregations are arranged without
reference to the boundaries of male territories.
ro
Or
Reproductive Behavior
territorial boundaries of Zalophus as compared with Callorhinus.
MAINTENANCE BEHAvIOR.—Once a bull is established on territory,
it does relatively little fighting. A territory rarely borders more
than two or three others, and established neighbors are not con-
cerned with displacing each other, but merely with maintaining
their own territories. Territorial maintenance activities consist pri-
marily of loud and frequent vocalizations, and of displays with
formalized postures and movements. The loud and incessant bark-
ing of a territorial bull continuously advertises his presence and
broadcasts his intent to defend his territory. When not aimed at
any specific bull these barks are relatively slow, but if the territorial
bull is moving toward another male, the rate of barking increases,
apparently signifying a more intense and directed threat.
Pairs of established bulls regularly perform a stereotyped boundary
ceremony, reaffirming their mutual territorial limits. They rush
toward each other, barking rapidly, with vibrissae extended an-
teriorly. Just before reaching the boundary between their territories
they stop barking and fall on their chests, open their mouths widely,
shake their heads rapidly from side to side, and weave their necks
laterally at a rate much slower than the head-shaking which is simul-
taneously going on. They then rear themselves to maximum height,
twist their heads sideways, and stare obliquely at each other. ‘Three
stages of this ceremony are shown in Figures 13, 14, and 15. The
process is ritualized, and the animals do not touch each other; if
they happen to be unusually close together they skillfully avoid
contact. Sometimes during the boundary ceremony the two bulls
partially cross the boundary and stand side by side, facing opposite
directions, making feinting jabs at the chest and flippers of each
other. The oblique stare is often the last component of the display
prior to the bulls’ separation, or prior to repetition of the entire
head-shaking, neck-weaving sequence.
TEMPORARY INTRUDERS.—The activities associated with territorial
maintenance by established bulls are not continuous. Periods of
alertness and movement are interspersed with intervals of sleep,
often at apparently preferred sites within the territory. During
these intervals of male inattention, which may last for several hours,
neighboring territorial bulls show no interest in entering the un-
tended territory. Non-territorial, wandering bulls, however, often
move cautiously into a territory at such times, usually “hiding”
26 The California Sea Lion
among the resident females. These intruders may be fully grown,
or they may be small animals the same size as the females. They
usually approach from the landward side, having landed at some
undefended place and wandered inland to the rookery, although
sometimes they enter from the sea. They do not vocalize, nor do
they challenge or otherwise disturb the bulls in adjacent territories.
Often they show sexual interest in the females, and may attempt to
mount them. When one of these intruders is discovered by the resi-
dent bull, a vocal threat is ordinarily enough to send him rushing
away. Sometimes the intruder remains undiscovered for hours, and
we observed one that remained quietly (apparently unnoticed) ina
territory for three days. Intruders seem to remain undetected as
long as they are visually inconspicuous and do not respond with
threatening or rapid movements to the activity of the resident bull.
Thus, there is a premium on their behaving very much like females.
Perhaps because of poor aerial vision bulls do not readily recognize
these intruders as males.
Early in the breeding season, most of the temporary intruders
are fully grown bulls, evidently from the hauling grounds. ‘Toward
the end of the season, they are more often young males about the
size of the females. A corresponding decrease in the number of
adult males on the hauling-ground population occurs as the season
progresses. If fully grown and allowed to remain for many hours
without being challenged by the resident bull, a temporary in-
truder may gain enough confidence to stand its ground briefly and
return the threat of the resident. But although they sometimes
fought with the resident, we never saw the temporary intruders
successfully establish themselves as territorial bulls.
The frequency of these intrusions is surprisingly high, evidently
because the linear arrangement of the territories makes almost every
territory accessible to wandering animals from both the landward
and the seaward sides. During the period between 1100-1400 hours,
14 June 1965, we saw four temporary intruders enter seven terri-
tories at the study area. Later in the season, small males sometimes
dashed into the aggregations of females every few minutes, fre-
quently unnoticed by the resident bulls, and often unnoticed by us
until they bolted from among the females when the resident
threatened them.
Reproductive Behavior 27
Because these temporary intruders are transient, and almost never
defend their positions, we do not consider the territories to be
composite, nor the intruders to be “subordinate breeding males”’
as in Mirounga (Bartholomew, 1952:394; Carrick et al., 1962:173).
RELATIONS OF FEMALE AGGREGATIONS TO MALE TERRITORIES.— The
individual females on a rookery pay no attention to the territorial
boundaries maintained by the bulls. The bulls patrol the bound-
aries irregularly and make no effort to herd females into their
territories or to hold them there. Consequently the arrangement
of the female aggregations shows no regular relationship to the
territories of the males, nor is a given female confined by a bull to
any one territory. Therefore, the word “harem,” so firmly estab-
lished in the literature on pinniped behavior, is not applicable to
Z. c. californianus. Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1955) uses the German term
Herd, suggesting harem, for the Galapagos sea lion.
Although the females are relatively independent of bulls’ terri-
tories, the converse does not hold. Stable and organized territories
exist only when and where females are present, and shifts in the
females’ locations modify the territorial activities of the bulls. Such
shifts in the locations of the females occur erratically, with storms or
disturbances, and regularly, with the tidal and day-night cycles.
The tidal cycle in California has a duration of approximately 12
hours; the females generally stay near the water’s edge, shifting
back and forth on the beach with the tide. The territorial bulls
generally move back and forth with them; the lateral and seaward
boundaries of the territories are fixed but the inland boundaries are
less definite. At night, the females usually move back to the inland
edge of the beach and sometimes even go above the beach onto
vegetation-covered terraces. The territorial bulls tend to accompany
the female groups as they move inland at night, though they still
continue to defend the seaward edges of their territories. If the
females abandon a stretch of shore completely, as they sometimes
do in the face of a heavy surf, the bulls remain on the vacant beach
for only a day or so before deserting their territories. Occasionally,
however, one finds a stretch of shore on which bulls are widely
spaced and aggressive toward each other even though females have
not been ashore there. We do not know how long bulls maintain
their stations in such areas, or why they are aggressive there, but
not aggressive on the hauling grounds.
28 The California Sea Lion
NocturRNaL Activiry.—We frequently visited the rookeries and
hauling grounds at night, and on 30 June 1965 we maintained an
all-night watch at the study area. In the rookeries, activity was, if
anything, greater at night than during the day. Although some of
the females and many of the pups slept, sleeping seemed no more
common at night than during the day. All of the usual activities”
seen during the day also took place at night. Vocalizing continued
unabated. Some females went to sea; others returned to suckle their
pups. Pups played, wandered about, slept, or suckled. The terri-
torial bulls were somewhat more active at night than during the
day. They moved about more continuously and appeared more
zealous in their inspection of females and their chases of intruding
males. It seems probable that the heightened level of activity of the
bulls at night is associated with the fact that over-heating is not as
acute a problem at night as it is during the day.
In contrast to the situation on the rookeries, the level of activity
on the hauling grounds frequented by the non-territorial males was
much lower at night than during the day. Most of the animals on
the hauling grounds moved inland and slept.
Behavior of Females
GREGARIOUSNESS.— The polygynous so-
cial order of this species results from
two basic elements, the male territory
and the female aggregation, both of
which vary seasonally as do the relative
ageressiveness and the relative gregari-
ousness of male and female. Arriving
pregnant females are clearly attracted
to the vicinity of other females already on land. Although just
after parturition the heightened aggressiveness between females
produces a quasi-territorial pattern among them, one rarely sees
a female-pup pair isolated by itself. Almost always the females re-
main in aggregations, albeit at the height of pupping the aggrega-
tions become rather loose. Later in the season the aggressiveness
between females diminishes markedly and the females and their
pups are nearly always found concentrated in aggregations. It
should be emphasized that throughout all of the female’s activity,
some degree of gregariousness persists.
Y
Zi jos pik
7
Fic. 13. Figures 13, 14, and 15 show typical patterns of movement in the
boundary ceremony of adjacent territorial bulls. In this photograph the animal
on the left has just lunged forward and both animals are shaking their heads
from side to side.
Fic. 14. Boundary ceremony continued; the oblique stare
Reproductive Behavior 29
ARRIVAL AT THE ROOKERY.—Pregnant females apparently land at
the rookery only a day or so before giving birth. They are noticeably
bulky, walk with relative difficulty, and are restless and irritable.
Sea lions are conspicuously vocal, but the pregnant females are very
quiet and rarely if ever vocalize.
The bulls generally pay little attention to the individual females as
they accumulate on the rookery, but sometimes they bark at them
and approach and sniff at their noses. Infrequently, a bull will
attempt to nuzzle a newly arrived female’s flanks or rump. The
females generally ignore or avoid the bulls, but sometimes they
react to the approach of a bull with open-mouthed threats or by
retreating. Only infrequently do bulls attempt to “herd” females
into their territories by threatening or blocking their way. When
it does occur, such behavior is much less vigorous than that of
Callorhinus bulls (Bartholomew, 1953). We never saw Zalophus
bulls make either prolonged or effective efforts to retain females in
their territories. Usually a territorial bull gives no response to
arriving or departing females, even if in full view of the bull. In
this species we saw nothing which we could interpret to be a
Pivanenm.
AGGRESSIVENESS BETWEEN FEMALES.—Even though they are gre-
garious and almost always gather in groups, a conspicuous feature of
the behavior of Zalophus females during the the breeding season is
their aggressiveness. As mentioned above this aggressive behavior
is heightened considerably at the time of birth when parturient fe-
males are virtually territorial (cf. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1955:293) .
Most of the aggressiveness of the females is quite formalized and
despite its vigor and noisiness injuries are rarely inflicted. Typi-
cally, an aggressive female shifts about restlessly near her pup,
barking continually and rushing toward any neighbor that ap-
proaches too closely. She threatens by extending her neck and
twisting her head to one side with mouth open wide. The female
being threatened responds in a similar manner. ‘Thus, one often
sees pairs of females weaving their heads and necks back and forth
and vocalizing (Fig. 16). Sometimes the two animals freeze in
this head-aside posture for several seconds. If the contest is not
terminated at this stage, they may begin biting each other on the
necks or flanks and sometimes they roll and writhe on the ground
like wrestlers, but inflict no wounds. Finally, one of the individ-
30 The California Sea Lion
uals withdraws a few feet and the squabble dies away. These inter-
actions are very common and, since they disturb adjacent females,
tend to be contagious. Sometimes three or four, or even more, fe-
males become involved. But the aggressive displays are brief
(a minute or less) and usually result in nothing more than minor
shifts of location. If the aggregation is startled, as by the alarm call
of gulls, the resulting changes in position of the females stimulate
a great Many aggressive interactions.
We have seen no evidence of stable dominance hierarchies, as
occur in Mirounga females (Bartholomew and Collias, 1962:8).
The absence of such hierarchies may be related to the greater mo-
bility of Zalophus and the resultant decrease in numbers of con-
tacts between the same individuals; Mirounga females lie almost
immobile near each other for several days. Transient dominance-
subordinance relations between pairs of Zalophus females do occur:
pregnant females, females without pups, or females en route to the
sea usually behave subordinately.
AGGRESSIVE VOCALIZATIONS.— There are at least four vocalizations
associated with the inter-female aggressive behavior. The com-
monest is the bark which is similar to that of the bull but higher
pitched. It identifies and localizes a female and serves as a relatively
mild, long-distance threat. When two contesting females are close
together, three intergrading vocalizations are used. The least in-
tense of these is a squeal which is of variable pitch, sometimes quite
high. It is made with wide-open mouth and usually accompanies
the head-weaving behavior sequences. The belch is a somewhat
more intense threat, a prolonged, harsh, gagging sound, usually
accompanied by forward thrusting of the head. The most intense
vocal threat is an irregular growl which has a harsh, erratic, aspirate
quality and is often used during actual strikes with the head or
during biting.
“PEACE-KEEPING” BY BULLS.—The noisy squabbles of females some-
times evoke a response from the nearest territorial bull which begins
to bark loudly, moves rapidly toward the females involved, and may
attempt to sniff or nuzzle them. Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1955:292) inter-
preted this behavior in the Galapagos sea lion to mean that “Das
Mannchen duldet keinen Streit unter den Herdenmitgleidern” and
suggested further that bulls actively prevent females from fighting.
Reproductive Behavior 3]
Steller (1751; translation:203) ascribed similar meaning to the re-
actions of fur seal bulls to squabbles in their territories.
We saw nothing in our study to which we would apply such
interpretations. Bulls react in a similar “peace-keeping”? manner to
any disturbances within their territories. For example, to young in-
truding males or especially noisy pups at play. It is reasonable to
assume that this response of bulls is simply a general reaction to
heightened activity in their territories. ‘The bull’s approach may
temporarily halt the fighting between females, by frightening them
into fleeing a short distance, but it certainly does not pacify them.
Parturition
Our daily censuses indicate that most of the pups were born dur-
ing June. Pregnant female sea lions are easily recognized by their
distended abdomens and laborious locomotion. Although we
watched them carefully throughout the pupping season we wit-
nessed only two deliveries, one at 1715 hours on 20 June, and one
at 1900 hours on 22 June 1965. Since new pups were present on the
rookery almost every morning during the pupping season, we con-
clude that most deliveries occurred at night.
During parturition, the females became increasingly restless as
they passed through the first stage (vaginal dilation) and fre-
quently turned to nuzzle the perineal region, sometimes circling
about like a dog chasing its tail. During this stage both the females
we observed uttered a few, brief pup-attraction calls (see section on
mother-young recognition) , but otherwise were silent. This vocali-
zation was directed toward the perineum, as if in anticipation of
delivery.
Both deliveries were by anterior (cephalic) presentation (cf.
Slijper, 1956:42). In one birth which was timed, the second stage
lasted 2.5 minutes. More than two minutes were required for the
pup’s head and shoulders to appear, and then movement was rapid
as the thorax, the thickest part of the spindle-shaped fetus, was
delivered. Thereafter, the hindflippers appeared almost immedi-
ately. The umbilical cord was broken, in both cases, by the time
the pups were delivered. The third stage (placental delivery) oc-
curred after 17 minutes in one case, and did not take place for at
least one hour in the other case.
$2 The California Sea Lion
The new pups lay on the rocks quivering for a few seconds, then
began to struggle weakly, made feeble attempts to stand on their
foreflippers, and succeeded in doing so within a minute or so.
They shook themselves like dogs several times during the first few
minutes, just as wet adults do, and soon began vocalizing.
Courtship and Copulation
Females come into estrus about two weeks after parturition.
The vulva of an estrous female becomes pink and slightly edema-
tous, and is therefore much more conspicuous than in other females.
Courtsuip.— Territorial bulls are relatively inactive in courtship.
They do not examine the females that happen to be in their terri-
tories with any regularity or system. However, bulls often attempt
to sniff the noses of females that are moving about, and will nuzzle
persistently at the genital area of any female that is lying prone
and is at all receptive. Sometimes a bull will move about a female,
barking and shaking his head, much as in the formal aggressive
encounters between males. When used in courtship, however, this
behavior assumes a rather tentative quality and may culminate in
the bull’s rubbing his whiskers along the female’s body and nipping
at her shoulders and sides (Fig. 17). Such a courtship performance
by the bull is by no means invariable and is always less vigorous
than in Callorhinus.
When a Zalophus female comes into estrus she usually solicits the
male to mount her. This contrasts sharply with other pinnipeds
in which the male plays the active role. The Zalophus female ap-
proaches the bull, and by characteristic submissive postures and
languorous movements (estrous display) invites sexual attention.
An estrous female lies prone in front of the bull, on her side, belly,
or back, and repeatedly presses her body against his. She executes
slow, writhing movements on the ground, rubbing against the
bull, stretching, and looking up at him. Sometimes while making
these twisting movements she slithers across the back and shoulders
of the male, almost as though mounting him. Usually, but not
invariably, the male soon begins sniffing at her genital region. She
responds by arching her back upwards and spreading her hind-
flippers. The male generally mounts after a few minutes, though
sometimes courtship continues for an hour or longer.
Fic. 15. Boundary ceremony continued; elevated posture with head shaking
Fic. 16. Open-mouthed threats by females
Reproductive Behavior 33
CoPpuLATION ON Lanp.—In copulation the female lies prone,
usually on her belly, and the bull mounts her from the rear. ‘The
front limbs of the male are short and the female is relatively large,
so even when the bull supports himself on his foreflippers, his chest
and belly rest on the female (Fig. 18). Occasionally a bull rests his
entire weight on the female and clasps her with his foreflippers.
The bull’s pelvic thrusts are intermittent. He usually mounts and
dismounts the female several times before copulation is terminated.
Between mountings the female resumes estrous display. The male
may cover her when she is lying on her side or even on her back and
make pelvic movements. These copulatory activities, including
mountings during which the pair rub their necks together, may con-
tinue for an hour or more and the pair may even fall asleep for a
few minutes while lying on or against each other. We were unable
to tell whether or not intromission occurred during all the mount-
ings. In all instances observed, copulation was actively terminated
by the female; she raised her head and forequarters, bit at the neck
of the male and pulled free, even though the bull was often still
making pelvic movements.
Once a copulation has been terminated by the female, her behav-
ior changes abruptly. She rebuffs the approach of her former
partner or other sexually interested males with jerky, spasmodic
movements of the head, not seen at other times, and also actively
retreats from them. We never saw a given female carry out more
than one series of estrous displays, nor participate in more than
one prolonged copulatory series, nor remain sexually active after a
completed copulation. However, if the first male solicited by an
estrous female is lethargic or unresponsive, she sometimes moves to
an adjacent territory and directs her displays at the male there.
In one case a male repeatedly mounted a female over a period of
about 1% hours, but for some reason his activities were inadequate.
The female then moved to another bull in an adjacent territory
and courtship and copulation continued for an additional 15 min-
utes before she became unreceptive.
For the most part sea lions show no response to or apparent inter-
est in the behavior of a sexually active pair. Once (13 July 1965),
however, we observed a female swim up to a pair which were cop-
ulating in the water, sniff at the head of the male, and then nip
him gently first on the flank and then on the neck. He made no re-
34 The California Sea Lion
sponse other than to sniff at her nose when she approached. On
another occasion (4 July 1965), two females on land were simul-
taneously courting the same territorial bull. During a period of
about half an hour he repeatedly mounted each female, sometimes
lying across both of them at the same time. Finally, he completed
a copulation with one of the females which then bit him on the
neck, pulled free and moved rapidly away. The male followed her
for about 10 meters, but she rebuffed him with the spasmodic head
movements referred to above. After about three minutes the male
returned to the second female who promptly resumed her sexual
displaying. The male mounted her and copulation proceeded.
After 5 minutes the second female bit the neck of the male and
pulled free and moved away. The male did not follow her.
During a prolonged courtship, a bull may leave the female and
exchange vocal threats with another bull, but he becomes relatively
unresponsive to his neighbors while he is sexually engaged. Sexual
behavior takes precedence over aggressive behavior; we never saw
a bull interrupt copulation to engage in aggressive interactions with
another bull.
COPULATION IN THE WATER.—Since many bulls’ territories are
partly aquatic, and since females usually stay near the water's edge,
copulation often begins when the two animals are partly in the
water. If in shallow water, copulatory behavior follows the same
general sequence as on land, except that the female rests on the
bottom under water and must occasionally lift her nose to the
surface to breathe.
We also observed a number of copulations in deep water, where
neither the male nor the female was in contact with the bottom.
Courtship, usually brief, is similar to that on land except that the
animals are swimming. To mount, the bull clasps the female with
his foreflippers, holding her against him. The pair sometimes roll
over in the water, float on their sides, or sink entirely under water
and wash back and forth in the surge, breathing only intermittently.
Judging from its frequent occurrence and duration, aquatic copu-
lation appears to be successful in Zalophus, a point that has been
much argued for Callorhinus (Bering Sea Tribunal of Arbitration,
139D)e
MOTHER-YOUNG RELATIONS AND
ONTOGENY OF PUP BEHAVIOR
Perinatal Behavior
INNIPEDS are among the most precocial
Pp of mammals (Nice, 1962:17) and Zalo- 4,
phus conforms to this generalization. The
pups have their eyes open at birth and within
10 to 15 minutes after delivery they can carry
out highly coordinated motor activities. For
example, a newborn pup can shake itself like
a wet dog, scratch with the hindflippers, rub
its flanks with the foreflippers, and use its
incisors to nibble and groom the pelage. Walking becomes coor-
dinated within the first 30 minutes. Vocal interchanges with the
mother begin at birth, and the pup almost immediately develops
the capacity for effective vocal communication.
Post PartuM MoTHER-YOUNG INTERACTIONS.—Shortly after delivery,
the female begins nuzzling her newborn pup, rubbing her vibrissae
over it and (perhaps incidentally) brushing away bits of fetal mem-
brane that may be sticking to its pelage. She repeatedly lifts the pup
to her side with her teeth and nips at its body. Although this
nipping and nuzzling continued for at least 10 minutes, we never
observed a female lick her pup (contra Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1955229D)
nor eat the fetal membranes or placenta.
Immediately after birth the pup and its mother begin an initial
series of vocal interchanges, which continue for 15 to 20 minutes
with little pause. The females make a bawling, trumpet-like pup-
attraction call and the pups respond with the mother-response call,
a quavering vocalization reminiscent of the bleat of a lamb. As in-
dicated previously, in the two births which we observed the females
began to utter the pup-attraction call shortly before the pup was
delivered. During the first several hours, these vocal interchanges
between pup and female are repeated a number of times. In the
intervals between these vocal interchanges the pair spends much of
the time sleeping. This post partum vocal interchange anticipates
20)
36 The California Sea Lion
the pattern of vocalizing that takes place between a female and her
pup when they are re-uniting to suckle after separation. Thus, it
may help establish the mutual recognition of mother and young
(see Collias, 1956, for a discussion of this problem in sheep).
We were surprised to observe that parturient females are nearly
as attentive to their neighbors as to their newborn pups. They bark
vigorously and make frequent, threatening dashes at adjacent ani-
mals. We saw one female leave her pup as soon as it was delivered
and rush over and threaten a neighbor, before returning to nuzzle
her pup for the first time. This aggressive, almost territorial, behav-
ior seems to have priority over all other activities at this stage, even
over preliminary interactions with the pup.
MOoTHER-YOUNG, Durinc First FEw Days.—Females are very pro-
tective of their pups for the first two to four days, and may even
refuse to leave them if approached by a man. Physical contact be-
tween mother and young is maintained almost continuously, the
female repeatedly tugs or lifts her pup to her side by the loose skin
of its nape and shoulder region whenever she shifts position slightly
or the pup starts to wander away. If sufficiently disturbed, by
weather or human activity, a female with a new pup will carry or
drag it in her mouth to a new location. Sometimes this involves
carrying the pup to sea. On 7 June 1965 we watched a female drag
her pup by its nape, first to the shore and then out into the surf
for perhaps 50 meters. The pup struggled frantically whenever
released from the female’s mouth, and several times it tried to crawl
onto her shoulders in a manner similar to that described by Kenyon
(1956). The female repeatedly caught the pup and lifted it above
the surface. ‘Twelve minutes after entering the water, they drifted to
shore about 400 meters distant from where they started. The female
helped the pup through the surge and onto the beach, and finally
the pair joined a group of females and pups in the new area.
Similar shifts in location occur on land. When a pup is less
than a week old, the female may carry or drag it; otherwise it follows
along behind her, vocalizing repeatedly. In a few unusual cases,
we observed females continuing to carry their pups cat-fashion
when 2-3 weeks of age. If the move involves climbing over rocks
or ascending ledges, a female tugs roughly at her pup and may
drop it several times. Partly as a result of this treatment, many
young pups have scratches and lacerations on their napes, although
Fic. 17. Neck-rubbing prior to copulation (top). Note conspicuous size dif-
ference between male and female.
Fic. 18. Typical copulatory posture (bottom). This picture was taken shortly
after Fig. 17.
Fic. 19. Pups playing in shallow water near shore |
Mother-young Relations 37
the scratches may also result from forceful rebuffing by other fe-
males. As pointed out previously, the female aggregations gradually
shift position with the tide and daylight, sometimes moving 30-50
meters on a flat, open beach. During these slow changes in location
the pups usually follow their mothers without being carried.
On sunny days when overheating is a problem, females make re-
peated trips to tide pools or the edge of the sea, sometimes every
hour or so, and get themselves wet. If pups are less than a week old,
the females take them along and drag them into the water. ‘The
pups flounder to shore and try to crawl out on land, but the females,
apparently reluctant to be separated from their pups, pull them
back into the water. This pattern, which appears to involve a
combination of behavioral thermoregulation and mother-young ties,
is probably the basis for the familiar folk belief that sea lions teach
their pups to swim. On first sight, it does appear quite like a “swim-
ming lesson.”
Sometime during the first day, the pup starts to suckle. As
pointed out by Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1955:296), there is no stereotyped
teat-searching behavior in Zalophus pups. Newborns nuzzle about,
apparently at random, until they find the site of one of the retracted
nipples, although later they learn to locate the site rapidly.
After approximately four days, the females no longer remain
continually with their pups. At first, separations may be very brief,
but as the season progresses, the females spend more time at sea,
and the pups begin to wander about the rookery, depending on
their mothers only for nutrition and not for protection.
Pup Behavior
The terrestrial locomotion
of pups during their first
months is much less variable
than that of the adults, con-
sisting of only two patterns: a
slow walk, and a faster gallop.
Both patterns are employed
shortly after birth, but pups become noticeably more agile as the
weeks progress.
Pups apparently can swim at birth, but their first efforts are
awkward, consisting of rapid rotary movements of the forelimbs
38 The California Sea Lion
while the head is held high above the water. This pattern is
strikingly similar to the terrestrial gallop, which also employs simul-
taneous rotary movements of the foreflippers.. Swimming skill de-
velops rapidly. Breathing coordination and use of the flippers im-
proves steadily and by mid-July many of the pups spend long
periods in the water (Fig. 19).
Pups spend much of their time sleeping. ‘Their sleeping postures
(Fig. 6) are similar to those of adults. If the terrain is flat the
sleeping pups orient themselves randomly, but on a slope they tend
to lie with heads uphill.
By mid-June, when many of them are two to three weeks old and
their mothers are no longer in continuous attendance, the pups
begin to aggregate in groups. Thereafter, these pods of pups become
a progressively more conspicuous feature of the rookery. Pups are
very gregarious: if one becomes isolated while sleeping, its first
act upon awakening is to rush frantically to the nearest pod. ‘The
pup pods may include as few as 5 or 6 or as many as 200 individuals.
The composition of a given pod is always in a state of flux; individ-
uals and small groups frequently join it or move away, and oc-
casionally the entire group moves off to a new location.
Typically, these pods are located near the female aggregations,
and shift with them diurnally and tidally. Pups aggregate in areas
where the territorial activity of the bulls is minimal. Thus, they
accumulate on the edges of the rookeries, among large boulders,
or beneath overhanging ledges. Bulls pay little attention to pups,
although sometimes it appeared that a bull would slow somewhat
before rushing through a pod, permitting the young to scramble
out of his way. We saw very few pups run over by bulls.
Pups appear to be less sensitive to heat and more sensitive to cold
than adults. They show no tendency to keep continually wetted
on warm days, and unlike adults, when they are wet, they often
shiver and repeatedly shake water from their pelage. From these
responses we infer that the young animals are poorly insulated and
dissipate heat readily, as is the case with Callorhinus pups (Barthol-
omew and Wilke, 1956:331; Irving et al., 1962:276) .
Pup Vocaization.—The only vocalization produced by newborn
pups is the bleating mother-response call, but after they become
older and form pods, they begin to use three other intergrading
vocalizations. If a pup is struggling, as during play, it occasionally
Mother-young Relations 39
emits an explosive, unvoiced cough, or a rapid series of them which
sometimes become voiced. If a pup becomes agitated, it often
prolongs the voiced portion of the series, and produces a relatively
high-pitched alarm call. In early August, when many pups are
about six weeks old, they begin to develop the adult-like bark, a
threat which seems to arise as a segmented version of the alarm call.
All three of these vocal patterns have social significance, evoking
various responses from nearby pups and adults. We shall discuss
vocalization further in a separate paper.
Pray oF Pups.—The play of sea lion pups resembles that reported
for northern fur seals (Bartholomew, 1959). From the time a pup
first joins a pod it engages frequently in elaborate play behavior
which appears to have no immediate relevance to nutrition, repro-
duction, or protection, but may adumbrate adult behavior.
Individual pups exhibit many playful motor patterns: rocking
laterally from one foreflipper to the other, rolling onto the back
and waving the flippers in the air, and elevating the forequarters
while simultaneously bending the neck back sharply and peering
about over the back.
Most of the pups’ play is social, however. Sometimes two or three
pups rub their heads, necks, and bodies together, rotate their
vibrissae forward, and touch noses. They often nip each other's
faces and forequarters, or gently interlock jaws. Some of these ac-
tivities assume the stereotyped configurations of adult aggressive
interactions. For example, open-mouthed threats, feinting lunges at
flippers and necks from a prone posture (Fig. 20), and mounting
with sketchy copulatory movements are frequently seen.
Sometimes large groups of pups engage in contagious patterns of
play. A pod may travel en masse through the rookery, moving er-
ratically and without regard to the bulls’ territories. One or two
pups often appear to lead these mass movements temporarily, while
the others gallop along behind, frequently stopping to nip each
other or roll on the ground in a brief scuffle. These wandering
groups seem to be attracted to conspicuous objects in the rookery
such as dead animals or tide pools.
By the time pups are about six weeks old they make short explora-
tory trips inland from the rookery singly and in small groups, sniff-
ing and rubbing against boulders, plants, or concealed human ob-
servers. As the season progresses they wander more and more widely,
40 The California Sea Lion
venturing along the coast and out to sea for short distances. By
late July the tide pools and sheltered inlets in the vicinity of the
rookery are often filled with bobbing pups, engaged in protracted
and elaborate aquatic play (Fig. 19). They submerge their heads,
peer about under water and chase each other over partially sub-
merged cobbles and boulders, frequently touching or nipping each
other. We watched one pup that seemed quite entertained by im-
mersing its muzzle repeatedly and blowing streams of bubbles from
its nostrils.
Pups also play with inanimate objects. They are strongly at-
tracted to pieces of the giant kelp (Macrocystis) which commonly
wash ashore, and they endlessly shake and toss them about. Several
pups may successively play with the same piece of kelp, and occa-
sionally two may tug on a piece at the same time. Small pebbles
and sticks are also picked up and held in the mouth, although we
never saw them thrown, as Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1955:297) describes for
Z. c. wollebaeki. Some pups energetically gnaw at rocks or banks,
occasionally packing their mouths full of sticky sand.
Mother—Young Recognition and Suckling
SUCKLING.—A pup soon learns the location of its mother’s four
retractable teats and switches from one to another as it suckles
(Fig. 21). The nipples emerge during suckling; whether extruded
actively or passively we could not determine; within a few seconds
after a pup releases a nipple, it retracts. The process of suckling is
surprisingly noisy, and smacking noises can easily be heard from a
distance of 10 meters. As Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1955:296) has described
in Z. c. wollebaeki, the pups use suction to obtain milk, rather than
massaging the teat with the tongue as some mammals do. Except
for lying on her side and exposing the area of the teats, the female
does not appear to assist the suckling pup; frequently she sleeps
while the pup feeds.
The usual pattern is for a pup to suckle steadily, with short
breaks, for about one-half hour, and then separate from its mother.
The mean, minimum, and maximum of five periods of suckling,
timed during daylight hours on 17 July 1965, were 33, 20, and 48
minutes. After such a period, the pup appears to be satiated and
wanders away from its mother. We seldom saw a pup which was
Mother-young Relations 41
more than two or three weeks old engage in more than one such
suckling period in a single day, but very young pups may suckle
intermittently for several hours. As discussed in detail later, pups
reunite with their mothers and feed at irregular intervals, seldom
more often than once each day or less frequently than once a week.
MAINTENANCE OF MoTuHeER-PuP TiEs.—We were able to mark four
female-pup pairs shortly after parturition (judged by fresh placen-
tas or by witnessing birth) , and we saw these pairs reunite repeatedly
during the three months following. We never observed among these
eight individuals any suckling which did not involve a mother and
her own pup. We also kept records of a number of other female-
pup pairs which although not marked immediately after parturi-
tion were assumed to be mother and young because of the female’s
attentiveness and the fact that she suckled the pup. We never saw
a female suckle any but her own pup nor did we see any female
accept two pups, either simultaneously or successively. Therefore,
we conclude that a Zalophus female recognizes her own individual
pup and nurses it to the exclusion of all others. Similar integrity
of the mother-young bond has been described for Callorhinus
(Bartholomew and Hoel, 1953:420-21) and Mirounga (Bartholo-
mew, 1952:391), although fosterage frequently occurs in Halichoe-
Misn(Eoea smith), 1965:73=74):.
Mother-young recognition probably becomes mutual by the time
a pup is about two months old, but prior to this, the integrity of the
pairs appears to depend on the female. Young pups respond posi-
tively to any searching female, but the female rebuffs all pups but her
own. The usual rebuff is an open-mouth threat but sometimes the
female seizes the pup in her teeth and tosses it randomly into the
air. This maneuver is usually performed awkwardly and sometimes
the pup is thrown against the female instead of away from her. ‘The
rejection is obvious, however, and the pup always retreats rapidly.
MorTHER-YOUNG ReEcoGNITION.—When a female comes ashore and
starts searching for her pup, she vocalizes repeatedly, approaches
any pups which respond, and appears to examine them both olfac-
torily and visually. When she finds the proper pup the process of
mutual identification generally takes only a minute or two, and
suckling begins shortly thereafter. Since the reuniting of mother
and pup usually takes place before the female has been ashore long
enough for her pelage to dry out (5-15 minutes), we infer that
42 The California Sea Lion
this behavior has a high priority when a female returns from feed-
ing at sea.
A rookery may contain hundreds of females and pups. ‘The
probable clues by which females and pups might identify each
other individually are few. Because we could not perform experi-
ments without disturbing the group we were studying, we have
attempted to assess the relative importance of several probable
recognition factors by repeated observations of the process under
undisturbed conditions.
1) Geographic location: Neither females nor pups appear to be
tied to a particular geographical site. Newborn pups may be moved
from one place to another by their mothers; older pups move with
their mothers for long distances, and the reunion and suckling of a
given pair occur at different sites at different times. From these
facts we conclude that strict geographic localization within the
rookery is not a primary factor in bringing a Zalophus female and
pup together, and that there is no one place to which a given pair
returns for reunion. Geographic localization could, however, offer
an important preliminary clue if, after each trip to sea, the female
returned to the approximate site at which she last suckled her pup.
Such a pattern would allow for shifts in location if the shifts oc-
curred when mother and pup were together. Our data suggest that
a pup, if it becomes separated from its mother for a prolonged
period, tends to remain at, or return to, the place where it was last
suckled. Pup Number 4, for example, was apparently unattended
by its mother from 14 to 20 June. During the period, it spent much
of its time near one site, where eventually its mother found it. How-
ever, the pair was never observed suckling at this location again.
2) Vocalization: We assume that mother and pup learn to recog-
nize each other’s voices during the repeated vocal interchanges
which begin immediately after birth and continue for several days
thereafter. On scores of occasions we have watched females land at
the rookery, stand with head high, trumpet the penetrating pup-
attraction call and then move toward a nearby pod of pups. Usually
a female will repeat the call several times per minute at irregular
intervals. Typically, one of the pups in the vicinity responds to her
call and moves directly toward her. Sometimes the pup is silent,
but it usually replies with the bleat-like mother-response call, and
then female and pup continue to vocalize alternately while moving
Mother-young Relations 43
toward each other. Several pups sometimes approach a vocalizing
female, but usually one is more vigorously responsive than the
others. At night when we had crawled into a pod of sleeping pups,
we several times saw individual pups suddenly rouse, reply to a
distant pup-attraction call by uttering the mother-response call,
and then move away to join the vocalizing female while the other
pups in the pod remained sound asleep. ‘The selectivity of response
of both mothers and pups was impressive, particularly in August
and September by which time the pups were 2-3 months old. Our
observations left us convinced that vocal discrimination plays an
important role in mother-young recognition.
3) Vision: Pups frequently respond positively to the sight of a
female if holding her head high, particularly if she is walking out
of the water onto the shore, and they may attempt to approach and
nuzzle her even if no vocalization has occurred. This response is of
low intensity and may occur simultaneously in several pups. Visual
clues, although obviously used to orient female and pup toward
each other, do not seem to contribute to specific individual identi-
fication. This is not surprising in view of the limited visual acuity
of the species and in view of the fact that identification takes place
at night as well as during the day.
4) Olfaction: From the frequency and persistence with which a
female nuzzles and sniffs at her pup, we infer that olfactory infor-
mation is important in close-range identification, as it seems to be
in goats (Klopfer et al., 1964). Once a female and pup have come
together, they invariably touch noses and sniff at each other; this
examination appears to complete the process of recognition. With-
out experimental evidence, we cannot say categorically that olfac-
tion is the essential element in mutual recognition, but from field
observation the case is convincing, particularly since the process
frequently proceeds to the nuzzling stage before pups are rejected
or accepted. We conclude that a female confirms the identification
of her pup by its individual odor.
5) Other clues: Other possible sources of information that could
be used for recognition are taste and touch. We cannot exclude
these, since the female and pup rub whiskers and may touch each
other’s noses, but these sensory modalities do not appear to be of
major importance.
44 The California Sea Lion
SEASONAL CHANGES IN MOTHER-YOUNG RELATIONS.—As the summer
progresses, pups and females spend progressively less time at the
rookery where parturition occurred, and they seem to spend less
time together on land. Since we often saw them depart together,
and swim along near each other, it appears that increasing periods
of time are spent together at sea during autumn months.
The mother-young tie may be maintained a year or longer. On
4 February 1950 on Islas San Benitos, Baja California, eight yearling-
sized individuals were seen suckling, as were four others at San
Nicolas Island on 23 April 1950, at a time when no pups had yet
been born. During June and July 1965 we repeatedly observed
suckling by individuals which could hardly have been less than
one year old (Fig. 22), although we cannot exclude the possibility
that these were young animals born out of season, in winter or
spring.
We often saw young animals (yearlings) accompanying females
at sea, swimming beside them and touching noses with them. On
15 July 1965 four female-yearling pairs were observed on land
as part of an aggregation of animals which included no pups. One
of the females moved away from the aggregation and the yearling
followed, very much like a pup would. When the yearling crawled
across the back of an adult, the two exchanged open-mouth threats,
an interaction which was never seen between a pup and an adult.
After moving 50 meters, the female lay down and the yearling began
to suckle.
Yearling members of such pairs are very active and independent.
For example, they will temporarily leave the adult and rush to the
sea during a minor disturbance. Even while suckling, they are
aggressive and may nip the mother or threaten her; they change
teats frequently and the intervals of suckling seem to be short.
We saw female-yearling pairs in both breeding and non-breeding
ageregations. Presumably the yearlings are not exclusively depen-
dent on their mothers for nutrition, but we have no data on this
subject. Nor do we know if a female with a new pup continues to
maintain ties with the offspring born in previous years.
Our observations on suckling in yearlings are not unique. Both
Rowley (1929:18) and Orr (19656:19) have previously reported
instances of young Zalophus suckling long after they could reason-
ably be classified as pups.
POOR ie Be
LE
wll
Fic. 20. Pups play-fighting (top). The pup on the right has just lunged
forward in a manner similar to that of a fighting bull.
Fic. 21. Female suckling pup on anterior teat (bottom) .
Fic. 22. Female suckling a yearling
QUANTIFIABLE ASPECTS OF THE
BIOLOGY OF ZALOPHUS
Population Dynamics
HE previous sections of this paper have
developed a qualitative picture of the
behavior of Zalophus and we can now at-
tempt to quantify some aspects of its biol-
ogy.
PopuLATION FLucTUATIONS.—The popula-
tion of sea lions on San Nicolas Island dur-
ing the breeding season remains in a state
of flux. Individual adult males arrive from
and depart for their non-breeding range at various times; pregnant
females arrive at different times; some of the animals are always
out at sea; bulls establish territories for a week or two then aban-
don them; pups are added in large numbers; animals change from
one rookery to another or shift between hauling ground and
rookery. Consequently, population estimates based on numbers
of animals on shore on a given day do little more than indicate
a lower limit for the total population.
The details of the distribution and numbers of Zalophus present
on San Nicolas Island during the 1965 breeding season are shown
in Table 1 and Figures 23 and 24. From May through September
the population of adult females fluctuated between 2700 and 4200,
but the number of territorial bulls never exceeded 408, and the
number of non-breeders varied between 1600 and 2600. The maxi-
mum number of pups were present on shore during early July and
since their population at this time was a little over 3600, there were
at least 3600 breeding females based on the island during the breed-
ing season. The total population, excluding pups, varied between
4100 and 5900. These last figures are more than twice those for the
summer of 1949 (Bartholomew, 1951) but only slightly larger than
those for the summer of 1958 (Bartholomew and Boolootian, 1960) .
The relative changes in numbers in the different age and sex
classes (Fig. 24) present on San Nicolas Island during the summer
45
The California Sea Lion
46
‘pautquios sotjtTeoo] qusoIe[pe OF s[VIOT, ..
66FE Org <c& OF GEIl 0 064 OF9 C6IT sdng
9664 OL ie ai wil Oss L801 696 $606 sy[npe [RIO J,
9641 006 0 Ost 0 OLE OF Gg OGL S19 paa1q-UON ‘dag
GOS OSr 04 097 051 04 OgOT 0L8 COSI so[eWoy J[Npe surpso.1g ial
99 1% il 3 ig 0 LI «Pl +8 S][NG [RILO}AI9 T,
GLOg Lob G 093 «+0 OST $94 a OB O rere sdng
OLIF ip OB @ O16 656 98 +06 06 G&&I sy[npe [eIO],
116 8Il 0 % © 006 891 OL Of O46 “eg S1apaa1q-uoN “ony
G03 0c9 8g 09g O Ol O8L COM OGIO OSII so[eWoy INpe surpsdig 6
99 Oi 8 0 0 0 «ll i 0 LI S][ING [RIO J,
FO9g OG co 18 #06 IL 066 Oc 9F 006 OGI féelc sdng
ILLS 998 146 64 OS 89 699 49 26 Ler 801 9896 sy]npe [R10],
6841 00g 0 0 0 Og O86 WwW @4 Or © O8F s1apaaiq-UoN Ayn
L199 Kose (2 WH 8 8 Gs OR 8 We Ol Waa So[BWoy J[NPe SUIps.ig 9-F
GTg 6r OL, G 3 ¢ Ww O08 Gl g ILI s][Nq [eILO} A139. T,
6983 4 W i O42 Me “és OWX6 WOR Ce G7 WG sdng
1849 Olfl 82 GG 6 OP 8201 Ser OL O68 266 OTAT sy[npe [eI0],
Gh9S 0901 0 0 0 Ole O84 Of GG OL. sOOGF s1opaoi1q-UoN oun
8313 062 Go 8F OS O01 096 OS OF Gég O61 O80I SoVUlay WPNpe Surpoaig 1
80F O, 8 L i 0 % 4 8 Ol 6 Olé s[[Nq [ILOILA19,T,
[eIOL, @@ WO Op Gh Wr GB WS Ge We ail Wil ssvD aed
SOT}I[VIOT
SHILITVOO'T YOA CZ AUN AAS
G96] JO NOSvaS DNIGAANG AHL WLIW GNV ONTUAC ‘VINAOATIVD ‘GNVIST SVIODIN Nv§ NO snupiusofyps snyqdolvz 10 SNOILV1Nd0d
1 WIdVL
Biology of Zalophus 47
of 1965 are consistent with what is known of seasonal changes in
behavior and distribution of the Zalophus of the coasts of California
and Baja California. Most of the pups were born during May and
June and the increase in numbers of pups during early summer was
merely an expression of the rate at which they were being added to
the population. During July the number of pups present on the
island decreased, presumably because of deaths and because the
pups were spending progressively more time at sea as they grew
older. The increase in pups during September is probably too great
to be accounted for by sampling error and may indicate that pups
born on other islands had followed their mothers to San Nicolas.
The Zalophus population on San Nicolas is larger in winter than
in summer (Bartholomew and Boolootian, 1960:368) and it is
reasonable to assume that the increase in all classes of animals,
except territorial bulls, that occurred during late summer was a
result of an influx of animals that were elsewhere during the breed-
ing season. The decrease in the number of territorial bulls in late
summer coincides with, and may be related to, the arrival in late
July and early August of a large contingent of male Zalophus at
Ano Nuevo Island, which lies 250 miles north of San Nicolas and
which supports a very large population of male Zalophus during
fall and winter (Orr and Poulter, 1965:394) .
The short-term fluctuations of the numbers of Zalophus on San
Nicolas during the summer are relatively small and resemble those
of Arctocephalus gazella (Paulian, 1964:34) more than those of
Callorhinus (Peterson, 1965b:20). Perhaps this is because Zalophus
and A. gazella feed closer to their breeding grounds than does
Callorhinus.
CHANGES IN LocaL DistripuTION.—Although the size of the sea
lion population on San Nicolas as a whole remained relatively
stable from day to day, the number of sea lions on any one rookery
showed erratic short-term variations. ‘This is shown by the periodic
censuses of the various rookeries (Table 1) and particularly by
the daily censuses of the study area (Table 2). Since we had many
marked individuals in the study area we know that the fluctuations
in animals there were caused by animals moving temporarily to
other nearby areas as well as by their going to sea. It is obvious
that because of the lack of close geographic ties, counts of a re-
The California Sea Lion
48
‘T 3[G¥.L, Ul UDATS a1” Sot}
-I[B0O] JUIIDJJIP oy} Je syeutuY Jo stoquInu oT, ‘spunois sul[NeYy puUe soT1oyxOOL
podruurd jo uoneoo] Surmoys ‘erIutozITED ‘purys] Se[OoIN uRg jo dew °€Z ‘I
(FAILINGONATY-NON) ONNO#D INI TNH >
(NOILVOFYIIV TAILINGOUATY ) AYFHOOY “er
YNOLNOD L001 00S ——
SYIVALSQVOH OFIVIENSNN ----
S004 CIDVIUNS ==
HoW3d
Was
INWHd3 14
d¢
ati
SONIYdS
GNVSNOHL
GNV ISI SVIOOIN NVS
Biology of Zalophus 49
stricted area give little insight into the performance of the popula-
tion of the island as a whole.
Morratity ON Lanp.—We found few carcasses of pups or adults
on land at any season. Since Zalophus stay very close to the water
during the breeding season, it is possible that some pup carcasses
wash to sea soon after death, which complicates any estimate of mor-
tality. However, the mortality rate of pups on land appeared to us
to be much less than that of Callorhinus on the Pribilof Islands,
which ranges between 5 and 16 per cent during the breeding season
(North Pacific Fur Seal Commission, 1962:5). For example, by 24
July 1965 only ten dead pups had been observed during the previous
six weeks in an area where approximately 250 pups had been born.
On San Nicolas during the 1965 breeding season, natural mortal-
ity of adults on land was negligible. Several of the carcasses of
adults which appeared on the beaches of San Nicolas showed signs
of human predation: buckshot wounds, or evidence of clubbing
or stoning.
Behavior of Individuals
‘TERRITORIAL BULLS.—By using scars and wounds for identification,
we were able to recognize and follow from day to day 14 individual
bulls in the study area. Although we accumulated many partial
records, we had difficulty in obtaining satisfactorily complete data
on length of territory maintenance; scar patterns disappeared or
were altered, and bulls remained partly submerged in the water
during daylight, making marks difficult to see. Five reliable and
complete records of duration of territory maintenance were ob-
tained during June and July. The mean duration was 9 days (3, 9,
9, 11, and 14 days). Our other records, although incomplete, sug-
gest that these data are typical.
We have no evidence that individual bulls changed from one
territory to another and we think that a given bull rarely assumes
territorial status more than once during the season. However, in
one instance a bull, marked by a projectile syringe in its flank,
moved from a territory in Area 1B to a hauling ground on July 14,
and later came back to his original territory.
ADULT FEMALEs.—Despite a fairly good technique for marking
females, we found difficulty in obtaining long serial records on them
50 The California Sea Lion
TABLE 2
POPULATION OF Zalophus IN STUDY AREA DURING AND AFTER 1965 BREEDING
SEASON
2 2 5 a 2
cy in LO) 2
See a. S we a.
© 2c a 8 8s a
oF ea es gE Se Bee ae
ow (5) pe OS 3 D x o =n 5} Oo
a ai te Ay aa = He a ed
June 3 3 39 40 13.0 July 3 3 * 30
5) 3 33 35 11.0 4 3 4] 44 13.7
6 4 = 2g 5 3 38 47 27
7 4 5 28 6 3 37 44 12.3
8 3 48 33 16.0 7 3 43 4] 14.3
9 3 51 34 17.0 8 3 31 36 10.3
10 3 46 32 15.3 9 2 38
11 2 47 20 23.5 12 2 29 40 14.5
12 2 * 26 13 2 21 29 10.5
14 2 38 20 19.0 14 2D 28 22 14.0
15 2 ‘ 18 15 1 30 25 30.0
16 2 = 17 2 38 22 19.0
17 Z 20 19 10.0 18 2 19 23 O15
19 4 43 48 10.8 20 2 15 26 RD
20 3 37 33 12.3 Ze 0 20 8 -
Al 4 39 40 9.8 24 2 32 20 16.0
22 4 31 21 7.8 27 2 19 21 9.5
24 4 36 30 9.0 28 I 24 9 24.0
27 2 40 32 20.0 29 1 25 11 25.0
28 2 56 37 28.0
29 2 52 51 26.0
Aug. 4 1 31 12 31.0
5 1 37 15 37.0
9 0 12 14 -
10 0 26 21 -
20 0 19 23 -
| 0 7 17 -
22 1 4 10 4.0
Sept. 14 0 12 19 -
* Census incomplete.
Biology of Zalophus 51
6000
5000
D
x
S 4000
=
=
3000
S
.
a 2000
S
=
1000
TERRITORIAL BULLS
JUNE JULY AUGUST SEZIE
Fic. 24. Zalophus populations on San Nicolas Island during and after the
1965 breeding season. See Table 1 for a more detailed analysis.
because their extreme gregariousness made the marked animals hard
to find and because they frequently changed locations in the
rookery. Eight females with pups were marked and followed for
approximately two months. To facilitate the assessment of the
proportion of the time devoted to various activities, a marked fe-
male was arbitrarily assumed to have been in residence for all of
each day on which she was seen in the study area.
The eight marked females were seen on shore in the study area an
average of 34 per cent of the days under observation, and were seen
suckling their pups on an average of 75 per cent of the days they
were known to be on shore (Table 3). However, the amount of
time spent on shore and the number of days on which they were
seen suckling changed as the season advanced. During mid-June,
when the pups were new, the females were on land an average of
50 per cent of the time, but thereafter the mean percentage de-
52 The California Sea Lion
TABLE 3
SUMMARY OF RECORDS FOR 8 MARKED FEMALES OVER A [TWO-MONTH PERIOD
DuRING THE 1965 BREEDING SEASON
Days Days Fraction of Days Fraction
Female of ob- days present observed of days
search served instudy area suckling suckling
A 24 9 0.38 3 0.33
B 25 10 0.40 9 0.90
Cc 26 12 0.46 11 0.92
D 29 11 0.38 10 0.91
1 24 11 0.46 10 0.91
Je 18 5 0.28 3 0.60
G 36 5 0.14 3 0.60
H 30 6 0.20 5 0.83
X 0.34 K 1075
creased markedly (Fig. 25). ‘The proportion of days on which they
suckled their pups declined sharply during July (Fig. 26).
PARTURITION AND CopuLATION.—From the population curves
(Fig. 24), it appears that most of the pups were born during June.
However, females that were obviously pregnant were common at
the study area from late May until about 10 July. More accurate
data on the temporal pattern of parturition will require observa-
tions in April and May and more extensive population estimates.
The first copulation we observed was on 16 June, and thereafter
copulation became progressively more common until the second
week of July. By 22 July, the number of sexually responsive females
had declined sharply as had the number of territorial bulls.
One marked female delivered during the night of 14 June and
subsequently came into estrus 15 days later, on 29 June. To judge
from this case, and the more general behavioral changes described
above, we infer that estrus occurs approximately two weeks post
partum. This period is similar to that in many other pinnipeds
(Harrison et al., 1952:439), but much longer than in Callorhinus
(Bartholomew and Hoel, 1953:420).
Pups.—The behavior of the 39 marked pups was classified in the
same way as that of females. Six pups were observed on fewer than
five days; 19 others were observed more often than this but were
seen to be suckled on fewer than three days; while the other 14 were
Biology of Zalophus 53
80
60
40
20
FEMALES
PER CENT OF MARKED AN/MALS
SEEN IN STUDY AREA
JUNE JULY AUGUST
Fic. 25. Changes in the populations of 8 marked females and 39 pups in the
study area. See also Tables 3 and 4.
seen more than five days and were suckled on more than three.
These figures suggest that fewer than half the pups caught and
marked at one rookery were actually resident, in the sense of being
permanently based there and being suckled there frequently.
Excluding the group of six pups seen fewer than five times, on
the average the marked pups were seen in the study area on 42
per cent of the days, and on 22 per cent of the days on land they
were suckled. Considering only the 14 resident individuals, the
comparable figures are 45 and 34 per cent, respectively.
The fraction of pups on land remained near the same level
throughout the season (Fig. 25). Of the pups marked in early June,
about one-third were observed in the study area on any subsequent
day through early August. The percentage for resident pups was some-
what higher than that of the non-residents. The fraction of the
days on which the marked pups were seen being suckled decreased
from a mean of 56 per cent in mid-June to about 15 per cent in
late June and to almost zero in August (Fig. 26).
PATTERNS OF Timinc.—The data on time on land suggest that fe-
males do not remain separate from their pups either for extended
or regular periods. Comparison of records of one female and her
marked, resident pup (Table 4) indicates synchronous timing of
54 The California Sea Lion
100
FEMALES
80
60
40
PER CENT SUCKLING
20
JUNE JULY AUGUST
Fic. 26. Changes in amount of suckling by marked females and pups in the
study area. See also Table 3.
behavior. The pup was observed on more days than was its mother,
but it did not remain in the study area rookery continually. The
intervals between the female’s visits to the rookery varied from
one to four days with no regular periodicity. The same statements
apply to other resident pups and to the adult females.
In summary, a pup appears to be suckled at irregular intervals,
which lengthen throughout the summer. Between times of suckling,
both female and pup are concurrently absent from the rookery for
considerable periods, particularly after the first ten days to two
weeks post partum. The possibility exists that the pair may remain
together elsewhere. ‘This pattern contrasts sharply with the pro-
longed suckling broken by long, regular intervals of separation, as
found in Callorhinus (Bartholomew and Hoel, 1953:421).
Ratio OF BREEDING FEMALES TO BREEDING MaAtrs.—The unex-
pectedly low level of site tenacity in Zalophus makes it difficult
to arrive at a satisfactory estimate of the effective sex ratio on the
rookery. The aggregations of females are not harems in the sense
of being groups controlled and maintained by the activities of the
Or
Or
Biology of Zalophus
TABLE 4
RECORDS OF OCCURRENCE OF A MARKED FEMALE AND HER MARKED PUP IN THE
Srupy AREA ON SAN NICOLAS ISLAND DURING CENSUSES ON 28 DIFFERENT DAys
BETWEEN 13 JUNE AND 7 AucGusr 1965
Jo of
days of
Days observation
Female seen 10 36
Female seen with pup 10 36
Female seen without pup 0 0
Pup seen 15 54
Pup seen without female 5 18
territorial bulls. The females and pups shift from territory to ter-
ritory and may even alternate between different rookeries. ‘The
females spend part of their time at sea. All of the females are not
in the same reproductive condition at a given time, nor do individ-
ual bulls remain on territory throughout the breeding season. Con-
sequently the raw census data shown in Tables 1 and 2 offer only
a rough index to the effective sex ratio.
The degree of polygyny in this species can be best appraised if
we can estimate the mean number of estrous females available
to each territorial bull. Unfortunately, because of factors listed
above, we can hope to achieve no more than rough approximations
of upper and lower limits for the number of females per territorial
bull (Fyg) during the month when breeding was at its peak (15
June to 15 July).
As shown in Table 1, at least 3604 pups were born on San Nicolas
during the 1965 breeding season. This means that a minimum of
3600 parous females must have come into estrous during the breed-
ing season. To this figure we can add at least 500 as a conservative
estimate of the number of nulliparous females coming into estrous.
The population of territorial bulls on the island during the breed-
ing season averaged about 320. If we were to assume that each ter-
ritorial male remained on his territory for the full 30 days, Foz =
4100
320
males remained on territory for about 9 days. Therefore, approxi-
= 13. However, we know from our study area data that the
56 The California Sea Lion
mately 1060 of them would have occupied territories during the
4100
1060
computations we can conclude that on the island as a whole the
ratio of estrous females to territorial bulls during the peak breeding
month was at least 4: 1 and could hardly have exceeded 13: 1.
As a check of the rough calculations based on the total Zalophus
population on San Nicolas, we can consider the small population
in the study area which was under almost continuous scrutiny and
for which we have relatively detailed information. Crude estimates
of F yp in this small sample during the peak of the breeding season
can be made if we assume that during this period (15 June to 15
July) the rate at which females came into estrous remained constant
and we know the following:
breeding season. Using these figures, Fp = = 4, From these
a. The daily mean of the number of estrous females (14).
b. The mean fraction of time the females stayed on the rookery
(OS)
c. The length of the peak of the breeding season (30 days) .
d. The mean duration of territory maintenance by bulls (9
days) .
e. The daily mean of the number of territorial bulls (3).
A consideration of the assumptions and data enumerated above
a
shows that Fy, = 5 => ic x e) = 5, which is similar to the mini-
C
mum possible figure obtained using the population as a whole. Frp
can be calculated for the study area using the pups as an index to
the number of estrous females. If we assume that the same number
of pups entered the study area as left it, approximately 48 pups were
born in the study area. If there were 48 pups, there were 48 parous
estrous females, and we can add to these an estimate of 5 nulli-
parous estrous females. If the mean number of bulls was 3 and
the average stay on territory was 9 days, there were 10 bulls present
during this period. Using these figures, Fp, = 5 which approximates
the minimum figure obtained from the population as a whole.
Just for completeness we can examine the uncorrected data in
Tables 1 and 2. During the breeding season the ratio of females to
territorial bulls varied from rookery to rookery and from time to
time. In the first week of June, the mean Fy, for the island as a
Biology of Zalophus 57
whole was 7; during early July it was 12. On the study area during
June and early July, Foz varied between 8 and 28, with a mean
Ol Mes
From the several approaches above, we can do no more than
conclude that the ratio of estrous females to territorial bulls during
the 1965 breeding season could hardly have been less than 4: 1
nor more than 14: 1.
DISCUSSION
LTHOUGH otariids having the ma-
jor morphological adaptations
for aquatic life shown by living forms
have been in existence since at least
the upper middle Miocene (Mitchell,
1962), many features of the social be-
havior of sea lions remain strikingly
similar to those of terrestrial mam-
mals. The particular pattern of social
behavior shown by Zalophus appears
to be determined by the interplay between gregariousness and ag-
gressiveness on the one hand, and the behavioral and morphological
adaptations associated with its amphibious mode of life on the other.
Gregariousness
Like other otariids, Zalophus are strongly gregarious when on
land. Although the species has traditional hauling grounds which
have been used for several decades at least, a local population may
shift from one area to another during a single breeding season.
Even with this mobility, Zalophus group themselves in crowded
aggregations separated by extensive unoccupied areas. ‘This sug-
gests that the local crowding so characteristic of Zalophus rookeries
and hauling grounds is not related to scarcity of suitable land
habitat but rather to gregariousness.
Terrestrial gregariousness in a wide-ranging marine mammal
should help to assure that breeding groups adequate for reproduc-
tion will occur. In the case of Zalophus, which is almost certainly
monestrous and which has a brief period of estrus, access of the fe-
male to a breeding bull during one limited period per year requires
some special behavioral adaptations, an obvious one of which is
gregariousness.
Another obvious selective advantage favoring extreme gregarious-
ness is that it increases the chance of detecting and escaping from
terrestrial danger. The presence of large numbers of individuals,
58
Discussion 59
any one of which by dashing toward the water can alert several
hundred others to possible danger, compensates for the limited
visual and olfactory acuity of the individual animals.
A geresstveness
Gregariousness and aggressiveness are not mutually exclusive be-
havioral attributes. Opportunities for intraspecific aggressive be-
havior are obviously abundant in gregarious forms because of the
frequency of contacts between individuals. In Zalophus, as in many
other highly gregarious mammals, patterns of intraspecific aggres-
sion are well developed and aggressive interactions contribute im-
portantly to the social structure. Aggressive behavior is more de-
veloped during the breeding season than at other times, and during
the breeding season it is more intense on the rookeries than on the
hauling grounds. In general, the aggressiveness of young animals
is less than that of the adults. Breeding females react aggressively
but with relatively low intensity toward other females, toward adult
males, toward juveniles of both sexes, and often toward pups other
than their own. The aggressive activities of breeding males are
much more intense than those of the females, but are focused
primarily on other males (including juveniles) .
Like other mammals (Matthews, 1964:30), Zalophus shows a
graded series of aggressive activities, extending from ritualized move-
ments and threats to savage fighting. Even during their most violent
fights, Zalophus bulls employ highly stereotyped movements and
postures. Consequently the various customary categories of aggres-
sive behavior are often difficult to distinguish. The classification
which follows has a large arbitrary component.
THREATS.—Both postural and vocal threats are frequently used in
encounters between males, females, and young, including pups.
‘These threats vary in intensity from vocal signals indicating loca-
tion or territory occupancy, to actions which are preliminary to
violent fights. ‘he basic threat is an open-mouthed thrust of the
head accompanied by a vocalization, the characteristics of which
vary with age and sex. This generalized expression of aggressive-
ness is extremely common and may be used by disturbed adults,
juveniles, or pups of either sex at any season and normally involves
pairs of individuals. The most common cause of this threat is the
60 The California Sea Lion
disturbance caused by an animal moving into or through a crowded
group. Usually the animal causing the disturbance merely moves
aside or retreats a few feet when it is threatened, but it may threaten
in return before continuing on its way. This threat usually marks
the total extent of an aggressive encounter, but during the breeding
season it is sometimes the prelude to a fight and in most cases an
animal that threatens gives the impression of being prepared for
such an eventuality.
Ficuts.—Fights in which serious wounds are inflicted occur most
often between males in relation to territorial establishment and
maintenance. However, females that are immediately post partum
may fight vigorously but they rarely inflict visible damage on each
other. Interactions which employ many of the motor patterns of
fighting are frequent among young non-breeding animals but these
are of low intensity and appear to be play.
Because they are so spectacular, the fights of otariid males have
been cited as the most violent that occur among mammals. Never-
theless, actual physical fighting occurs only under a limited set of
circumstances. Zalophus bulls fight only when territory mainte-
nance is involved. Bulls inflict nasty cuts and gashes, but we have
never seen a fight result in death. Postural and vocal threats precede
and are intermingled with attempts to inflict physical damage, and
some of the motor patterns involved in actual fighting have become
ritualized as described below.
RITUALIZED AGGRESSIVE BEHAvIoR.—A significant component of
the aggressive behavior of Zalophus bulls is ritualized in that it con-
sists of signals which do not of themselves serve as threats. The rit-
ualized aggressive behavior of Zalophus conforms to the original
usage of Huxley (1923) in that its evolutionary derivation appears
to be separate from its information content (cf. Lorenz, 1964:48).
This is shown with particular clarity by the stereotyped and
formalized boundary ceremony which is frequently engaged in by
bulls with adjacent territories. The vocalizing, head-shaking, neck-
arching, falling-prostrate, and oblique-staring patterns of the cere-
mony appear to be mutually recognized expressions of social status
and do not lead to fighting. Nevertheless, the individual elements
of the ceremony resemble the motor patterns used during fights: (1)
Head-shaking is much the same movement that territorial bulls use
when violently seizing and shaking an opponent. (2) The falling-
Discussion 61
prostrate maneuver is very similar to the forward lunge with pro-
tective appression of the foreflippers used by fighting territorial
bulls. (3) The recurved-neck posture resembles a posture frequently
seen during pauses between slashing bites. (4) ‘The vocalizations
used during ceremonies suggest prolonged versions of the grunts
and gasps of fighting animals.
In the absence of information on sea lions reared in isolation,
we cannot say whether these ritualized patterns are genetic or
learned, but the individual behavioral elements appear early in
ontogeny. Whatever the mode of transmission and maintenance,
the motor elements in the boundary ceremony closely resemble,
and appear to have been derived from, behavior used in fighting,
and represent examples of ritualized behavior in the sense of Huxley
(but not necessarily in the sense of Blest, 1961).
ONTOGENY OF AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR.—The precociousness of sea
lion pups, and the ease with which the behavior of animals of all
ages and both sexes can be observed at hauling grounds, allows the
ontogeny of aggressive behavior to be followed from infant to adult
with unusual clarity. The mock-battles of pups employ motor pat-
terns which clearly anticipate, in an unorganized way, modes of
ageressive expression used by adults. The prolonged, energetic
games of juveniles (aged one to five years) closely resemble the
serious threatening and fighting of adults, but the patterns are
still incompletely developed and appear unrelated to any imme-
diate goal other than play. Adult males on hauling grounds interact
almost exactly like territorial bulls, although less intensively. ‘Thus,
the maturing sea lion continually refines and standardizes the motor
patterns of aggressive expression.
Although these behavior patterns are performed frequently over
a period of years, they are actually play until the individual be-
comes an effective member of the breeding population. In sea
lions as in the polecats (Mustela putorius) described by Poole
(1966) , aggressive play consists of many activities that, in another
context, are vital to reproduction and hence extremely important
to survival. The transition from aggressive play to serious aggres-
sion is extremely subtle. For example, a bull when moving into a
rookery merely performs, with heightened intensity and persistence,
behavior patterns which, only a short time before, he may have been
performing playfully on the hauling ground. But as soon as he
62 The California Sea Lion
enters a rookery, these patterns of behavior determine whether or
not he will establish a territory and become part of the breeding
population.
The motor patterns of play and serious aggression are so similar
that in some circumstances we were unable to tell whether animals
employing them were playing or not, and sometimes one member of
a pair of participating animals may itself apparently briefly misin-
terpret the situation; i1.e., one member of an interacting pair reacts
playfully but the other reacts in a seriously aggressive manner. It
seems that even though the motor patterns involved in social inter-
action appear to require several years of practice before they are
performed accurately enough for them to have communicatory pre-
cision, the information content of even an accurate motor perform-
ance may be obscured except in the proper context.
EFFECTS OF AGGRESSIVENESS ON SOCIAL ORGANIZATION.—In Zalo-
phus the spatial organization and structure of the adult breeding
population are determined by aggressive interactions between in-
dividuals of the same sex. The interactions between females deter-
mine their location and spacing; the interactions between males
determine whether they can come ashore in the vicinity of the fe-
males and also fix their location and spacing once they are ashore.
Aggressive interactions also support the integrity of mother-young
pairs. Females prevent their newborn pups from coming into con-
tact with other females by maintaining territories around them-
selves and by vigorously rebuffing any pup but their own which
approaches. The pups at first appear to be attracted to any female,
but the aggressiveness which they evoke from all females except
their own mother strongly encourages them to react in a highly
selective manner to the scores of females they encounter. This situ-
ation must contribute to the rapid development of the mutual
recognition of mother and pup which is a basic component of the
social pattern in Zalophus.
The extreme development of male secondary sexual characteris-
tics in Zalophus is related to aggressiveness. ‘The participation of
a bull in reproductive activity depends upon his success in aggres-
sive encounters with other bulls, since these encounters determine
access to the females. Large size, thick neck pelage, and enlarged
canines obviously enhance success in fighting, and so a condition
exists which strongly favors the evolution and maintenance of
these attributes (see Evolution of the Social System) .
Discussion 63
The territorial bulls show no aggressive reactions to pups, but
their large size and rapid movements are an important source of
potential danger to the pups. After they are a few weeks old the
pups avoid active bulls. This has the effect of concentrating the
pup pods in parts of the rookery where territorial activity is mini-
mal and so contributes to the spatial organization of the rookeries.
During the non-breeding season when aggressiveness is greatly
reduced, spatial organization is almost completely random, and the
aggregations have no stable structure. ‘The low intensity, non-
reproductive aggressive interactions which are so common on the
hauling grounds appear to contribute to comfortable spacing. Sea
lions tend to form closely packed groups, and mildly aggressive
reactions simply keep the degree of crowding to acceptable levels.
SUPPRESSION OF AGGRESSIVE BEHAvIOR.—During the breeding sea-
son, nearly all interactions between adult Zalophus have aggressive
components and at least some of these must be suppressed for a
stable social structure to persist, and for reproduction to be success-
ful. Gregariousness balances the centrifugal effects of aggressiveness,
but for successful copulation or for suckling, individuals must be-
come at least temporarily tolerant of, or submissive to, others. As
would be expected, therefore, there are two circumstances in which
the usually aggressive reactions of Zalophus females are clearly sup-
pressed; when they seek out and suckle their own pups, and when
they actively solicit the attentions of and then copulate with a
sexually active male.
Territorial bulls never suppress their aggressive behavior com-
pletely. Even during courtship and copulation they often vigorously
threaten and nip at their partners. The acceptance by bulls of ritu-
alized signals in place of fighting at territorial boundaries may be
an example of partial suppression of overt aggression.
Amphibious Adaptations
Although California sea lions spend much
of their time on shore their adaptations for
aquatic life profoundly influence their social
behavior. ‘They are essentially coastal animals.
Large populations can be found on shore at
all times of the year. During the breeding season they appear to
have obligatory ties to land; there is no evidence that the young
64 The California Sea Lion
can be successfully born and suckled at sea. Copulation is usually
terrestrial, but aquatic copulations occur with some frequency so
this aspect of reproduction does not have to be terrestrial.
LimireD TERRESTRIAL Mositiry.—In pinnipeds the stability of
the social structure during the breeding season appears to be strongly
affected by their limited terrestrial mobility, which, together with
their sedentariness, allows many highly aggressive males to remain
physically close to each other without disruptive interactions. For
example, in those groups of northern elephant seals, Mirounga an-
gustirostris, which happen to breed on narrow beaches, the ordi-
narily stable social structure often becomes chaotic at high tide
when some of the females are partly awash (Bartholomew, 1952:
415). In this situation, the dominant male who is tied to the land
cannot move rapidly enough to prevent the subordinate males that
are swimming in the water from making rapid and repeated sorties
into his harem. As a result the social structure temporarily disinte-
grates into a confused mélange of aggressive and sexual behavior.
By analogy with this situation, it seems possible that the looseness
of the social structure in Zalophus as compared with Callorhinus
may be associated with the close association of its rookeries with
water. For example, in Zalophus, but not in Callorhinus, the male
territories nearly always abut on the water; the females remain near
the water’s edge; aquatic copulations are common; and aggressive
interactions between territorial bulls and males in the water are so
frequent that the social structure is subjected to repeated disruptive
influences.
SPECIAL SENSES.—Pinnipeds are faced with the difficult problem
of obtaining adequate sensory information from both air and water—
media with very different physical qualities. Judging by behavioral
criteria, visual acuity of Zalophus in air is limited both at close
range and at a distance. As previously described, only large moving
objects evoke responses and none of the social signals they employ
requires precise visual discrimination.
Although pinnipeds probably do not use olfactory clues under-
water, during the terrestrial phases of their behavior Zalophus em-
ploy olfaction in at least two circumstances; in mother-young recog-
nition and in identification of estrous females by bulls.
The marked development and conspicuous mobility of their
vibrissae suggest that tactile information is important to Zalophus.
Discussion 65
Certainly this is the case in social interactions on land—nuzzling
and nose rubbing are conspicuous in contacts between pairs or
groups of individuals.
Airborne acoustic signals are the primary means of social com-
munication in Zalophus. We know of no other mammal which
vocalizes as loudly and continuously as male Zalophus, and the fe-
males and pups are almost as vocal as the males. By means of vocal
signals bulls indicate their identity, status, location, and aggressive
intent; females summon and recognize their pups and express their
aggressiveness; pups reply to their mothers and signal to each other.
The overwhelming emphasis on acoustic signals is probably related
to the specialization of the other special senses for underwater func-
tion. Vocal signaling also compensates in part for the limited ter-
restrial mobility because it permits the animals to communicate
over considerable distances.
‘THERMOREGULATORY REQUIREMENTS.—Because of their amphib-
ious mode of life, pinnipeds are faced with conflicting thermoregu-
latory needs. They must retain heat while in the water, but must
be able to lose it while on land (Bartholomew and Wilke, 1956;
Irving et al., 1962). The conflicting demands for heat retention in
water and heat dissipation in air are particularly acute for Zalophus
because their warm-temperate and tropical distribution imposes a
severe heat load on them when they are on shore. ‘They have a low
thermal conductance because of their large size and thick layer of
subcutaneous fat. Consequently they face problems of overheating
when they are active on land, especially in direct sunlight. They
meet this probably in part by keeping wetted, thus taking advan-
tage of evaporative cooling, and partly by staying on a wet sub-
stratum to which they readily lose heat by conduction. It seems
likely that the thermoregulatory necessity for staying wet has been
an important selective factor favoring partly aquatic territories. As
discussed above, this has in turn affected the entire pattern of the
reproductive behavior in this genus. In addition, the high level
of nocturnal activity in Zalophus may be related to the avoidance
of overheating.
EFFECTS OF HUMAN DisTURBANCE.—On the islands where they
come ashore at the present time, Zalophus have no important ene-
mies except man. Human disturbance has frequently driven other
shore-breeding marine mammals nearly to extinction (Callorhinus,
66 The California Sea Lion
Enhydra, Mirounga, Arctocephalus) or caused them to abandon
their hauling grounds (for example, Eumetopias in the Pribilofs,
Kenyon, 1962). Yet, some of the islands utilized by Zalophus have
been occupied by Indians for several millenia (Meighan and Eber-
hart, 1953; Hubbs and Roden, 1964), and occupation of them by
man continues at the present time.
Although in the Channel Islands Zalophus appear to prefer
windward shores with extensive rocky shelves, they also use sandy
beaches, sheltered cobble beaches, and even talus at the base of
rocky cliffs for rookeries. Furthermore, site tenacity in Zalophus is
limited and contrasts sharply with the remarkably conservative pat-
tern of Callorhinus (Peterson, 1965b:93, 121). Even the breeding
population of Zalophus is mobile. If harassed by humans or dis-
turbed by storms, scores of animals may abandon a beach and move
to nearby areas and there reconstitute their social structure. Fe-
males and pups move as pairs, by sea or overland, and a day or two
after the females leave, the bulls abandon their territories. Other
bulls, meanwhile, have established territories in the areas newly
occupied by the females. These shifts may be local and temporary,
or wide-spread and prolonged: we observed the temporary move-
ment of one population as a result of our attempts to mark some
individuals; and military operations have disturbed San Nicolas
Area | so that during the past ten years the population there has
gradually diminished despite a total increase in the island’s popu-
lation.
This lack of restrictive site attachment appears to have contrib-
uted to the success of Zalophus in the face of disturbance by mod-
ern man, but we do not believe that Zalophus could withstand ex-
cessive, widespread exploitation, or that incidental disturbance is
harmless. The human population of California is competing in-
creasingly with Zalophus, and protection of their rookeries will be
necessary for continued survival of this valuable biological resource.
Evolution of the Social System
As Nutting (1891) pointed out, the breeding
patterns of pinnipeds can be arranged in a series aN
ranging from monogamous, relatively solitary
genera with little sexual dimorphism (Phoca,
Hydrurga) , to extremely polygynous, highly gre-
garious, and dimorphic genera (Callorhinus,
Arctocephalus). Between these extremes are a
Discussion 67
variety of intermediates: semi-gregarious genera (Leptonychotes,
Monachus), and others in which a harem system is partially de-
veloped (Halichoerus, Cystophora). The more polygynous species
copulate on land while the more nearly monogamous ones do so in
the water, and the breeding season tends to be more compressed
in the polygynous forms (Bertram, 1940:127). The degree of ag-
gressiveness between males, and gregariousness among females, gen-
erally parallels the degree of polygyny.
Sea lions (subfamily Otariinae) are generally assumed to belong
at the highly polygynous end of this series. Possibly, this may be
correct for Eumetopias (Mathisen et al., 1962:470; Daetz, 1959), for
Otaria (Hamilton, 1934; vaz Ferreira, 1961), and for the little-
known Neophoca and Phocarctos (King, 1964), although more
adequate data are needed for all of these genera. Zalophus, however,
can hardly be classified as one of the highly polygynous pinnipeds:
copulation is frequently aquatic, females aggregate without regard
to the territorial activity of bulls, male aggressiveness is relatively
reduced and territories are held only briefly (actually less than the
interval between parturition and estrous), temporary residence in
the rookery by non-breeding males is common, and the breeding
season is relatively prolonged. Admittedly, sexual dimorphism is
conspicuous and females far outnumber males on the rookeries (a
point to which we shall return). The social pattern of Zalophus
resembles that of Halichoerus in the eastern Atlantic, which 1s
rather loose and in which the degree of polygyny is moderate—both
frequently copulate in the water, bulls cannot hold females against
their will, male territories are short-lived and transient, and the
females are relatively independent and aggressive (Hewer, 1960) .
We suggest that these two species should be considered weakly
polygynous and placed midway in the behavioral series.
A reasonable hypothesis for the origin of polygynous social struc-
ture in pinnipeds can be formulated from the ideas originated by
Nutting (1891) and elaborated by Bertram (1940) and Bartholo-
mew (1952). The social pattern of the polygynous pinnipeds could
have evolved as a result of the production of a combination of
interactions between (1) their amphibious habits, (2) the gregar-
iousness of the females, (3) the aggressiveness of males during
the period of sexual activity, and (4) a basic pattern of reproduc-
tive physiology similar to most mammals, which permitted the ex-
68 The California Sea Lion
clusion of some males from the breeding population. Breeding
females tended to aggregate in crowded groups when on land while
the males were attracted to the vicinity of the females but, being
intolerant of each other, spaced themselves more widely. Thus, a
local imbalance in the sex ratio developed. The competition be-
tween males for location among the females led to this imbalance
being self reinforcing. It also led to a selection for the epigamic
and epideictic characters favoring success in aggressive interactions
and elaborate patterns of threat behavior. This distortion of the
effective sex ratio and exaggeration of sexual dimorphism could
continue only until it became so extreme as to prevent an adequate
reproductive rate.
This model suggests a possible mechanism whereby the system
may have evolved, but does not consider the functional advantages
conferred by this system which must have favored its selection
and maintenance over alternatives. Wynne-Edwards (1962:525)
has postulated that both gregarious breeding and polygamy are
homeostatic adaptations by which regulation of population size
is achieved and over-exploitation of the environment avoided.
Wynne-Edwards’ theory has been considered in relation to pinniped
colonial behavior by Coulson and Hickling (1964:509). We sug-
gest that this polygynous breeding pattern may also have other
selective advantages. For example, for a pinniped in which copula-
tion is restricted to land where locomotion and communication are
difficult, the polygynous system might well be advantageous simply
as an efficient mechanism of re-uniting the sexes at the proper time
each year. Thus, in a wide-ranging marine species of small total
population size, maintenance of a minimum reproductive rate could
be a problem, and colonialism and polygyny might have helped to
obviate it.
Our present concern is to evaluate these hypotheses and theories,
and relate them to the observed patterns of behavior of Zalophus. Is
the flexible polygynous system of Zalophus a stage in the gradual
progression toward a more rigid pattern like that of Callorhinus? Or
does it represent an evolutionary regression of the harem system,
as the animals gradually become more aquatic in their reproduction?
Bertram (1940:128), in reviewing the behavior of pinnipeds,
has proposed that the relatively sketchy harem system in Halichoerus
ee
is vestigial, and that the “.. . truly polygynous habit, which in the
Discussion 69
past was the cause of the sexual disparity in size and behavior in
this species, is now in the process of decay due to the growing inci-
dence of aquatic copulation.” ‘The same logic is applicable to Zalo-
phus. The alternative, that polygyny and its concomitant features
are just beginning to evolve, is less tenable since morphologic di-
morphism is much more highly developed in Zalophus than seems
warranted by the present, loose social pattern.
Coulson (1965:55) objected to the idea that colonial breeding
in grey seals is a vestigial behavior pattern, suggesting that if this
behavior were not serving important functional purposes, it would
rapidly disappear since “selection pressures against its persisting
are high.” We do not propose that all the selective advantages
which once favored the evolution of colonial breeding, polygyny,
and dimorphism have disappeared from the animals’ present en-
vironment. Instead, we suggest that some of the advantages, to
Zalophus and Halichoerus at least, may have been reduced, result-
ing in a partial regression of the harem system. An important factor
contributing to the relaxing of the social structure may be the in-
crease in frequency of aquatic copulation, which, as Bertram showed,
could obviate much of the terrestrial social behavior of pinnipeds.
Another potentially important factor for Zalophus is the relatively
high temperature on their rookeries during the daytime, which
apparently causes them to remain near the edge of the water. The
high level of mobility of males in the water makes prolonged main-
tenance of territories abutting on the water difficult. (Even in Cal-
lorhinus such territories are unstable, Bartholomew, 1953.) The
frequent visits of females to the edge of the water for thermoregula-
tory purposes impede the maintenance of stable harems and of
course make possible aquatic copulations. It is noteworthy that in
Callorhinus harem maintenance is relatively ineffective on the in-
frequent sunny days on which heat stress causes females to leave the
rookery and enter the water (Bartholomew and Hoel, 1953; Barthol-
omew and Wilke, 1956).
SUMMARY
His study of the natural history and so-
T cial behavior of the California sea lion
(Zalophus californianus) is based primarily
on observations made on San Nicolas Island,
California, during the spring and summer of
1965, but it draws on data acquired during
the past two decades on the islands off Cali-
fornia and Baja California and on the Galapagos Islands, Ecuador.
Zalophus are highly gregarious at all seasons. ‘They are primarily
coastal in distribution and haul out on shore throughout the year.
Although their primary locomotor adaptations are for swimming,
with the primary propulsive force being supplied by simultaneous
strokes of the front flippers, they are capable of effective locomotion
on land. Their terrestrial locomotion is variable and includes
walking, galloping, and striding with front flippers only. Despite
their short pelage they groom themselves extensively using both
front and hind flippers and occasionally the teeth. They also often
rub themselves, particularly their snouts, against each other and
against inanimate objects. ‘They have no special postures for defe-
cation or urination.
While on shore they appear sensitive to high air temperatures and
direct solar radiation and on warm days remain on the wet sand
and rocks at the water’s edge and frequently wet themselves down.
The visual acuity of Zalophus in air seems limited, but they
readily distinguish bold outlines and rapid movements. Auditory
and olfactory discrimination appear to be well developed.
Interspecific interactions with elephant seals, harbor seals, and a
variety of birds are common. Zalophus tend to be dominant in their
relations with harbor seals and young elephant seals, but not with
adults of the latter.
During the non-breeding season Zalophus appear to have no
stable social organization while on shore although there are obvious,
but presumably transient, size-related dominance relations. Except
during the breeding season there is a strong, although incomplete,
sexual allopatry. The adult males, but not the females or immature
70
Summary 71
animals, tend to move to or beyond the northern part of the breed-
ing range.
During the non-breeding season the escape reaction of Zalophus
is more pronounced than during the breeding season. At all seasons
their alarm response is highly contagious and is often evoked by
the alarm reaction of gulls associated with them.
Zalophus are highly polygynous seasonal (June and July)
breeders. Only territorial bulls participate in the breeding struc-
ture. The male territories almost invariably abut on the water and
some are completely aquatic. Bulls rarely hold territory continu-
ously for more than two weeks so that a given territory may be oc-
cupied by a succession of bulls during the breeding season. The
males establish and maintain their territories by aggressive behavior
involving almost incessant vocalization, frequent threats, and some-
times violent fights. However, adjacent territorial bulls do not
fight with each other, but maintain their status by stereotyped pat-
terns of behavior apparently derived from the motor patterns of
fighting. The females pay no attention to the boundaries of the
male territories and move freely from territory to territory. ‘Vhe
males do not attempt to hold the females so harems as such do not
exist.
Although the females are extremely gregarious they are suffi-
ciently aggressive toward each other immediately post partum that
for a few days they are almost territorial. The females are mon-
estrous and estrus occurs about two weeks post partum. Copulation
usually takes place on land but it often occurs partly or completely
in the water. Females apparently usually copulate only once during
estrus.
Parturition usually occurs at night. The pups are extremely pre-
cocious and can locomote and vocalize within a few minutes of
birth. Females are very solicitous of their pups for the first few
days and maintain almost continuous contact with them. They take
their pups with them when changing positions in the rookery, and
when going into the water to cool off they sometimes drag the
pups with them into the water. As the season progresses the females
spend progressively less time with their pups.
By the time the pups are a couple of weeks old they aggregate into
pods in the quieter parts of the rookery. They spend much time
in play both on land and in the water. ‘Their play anticipates many
72 The California Sea Lion
of the activities of the adults, particularly fighting and copulation.
They spend progressively more time in the water as they become
older.
Each female recognizes its own pup and suckles it to the exclusion
of all others. Mother-pup recognition involves location by vocaliza-
tion and final identification by olfaction. Visual clues appear to be
of minor importance. Mother-young ties are sometimes maintained
for a year or more and females are frequently seen nursing yearlings.
From May through September 1965, the population of adult fe-
males on San Nicolas Island fluctuated between 2700 and 4200,
but the number of territorial bulls never exceeded 408. The total
population on the island, excluding pups, fluctuated between 4100
and 5900. Slightly more than 3600 pups were born. Pup mortality
was very low compared with that of other highly colonial pinnipeds.
The mean duration of territory maintenance of individually
known bulls was 9 days and the longest was 14 days. The females
appear to spend about one day in three ashore with their pups.
Various methods of calculating the ratio of breeding females to
breeding males on the rookeries yield figures that indicate that the
ratio can hardly be less than 4: 1 or more than 14: 1.
The particular pattern of social behavior shown by Zalophus
appears to have been determined by the interplay between gregari-
ousness and aggressiveness on the one hand and the behavioral and
morphological adaptations associated with an amphibious mode of
life on the other. The roles of each of these are discussed as are the
relation of play to the ontogeny of aggressive behavior and factors
contributing to the evolution and maintenance of the polygynous
social structure of Zalophus.
Literature Cited
BARTHOLOMEW, G. A. 1951. Spring, summer and fall censuses of the pinnipeds
on San Nicolas Island, California. Jour. Mamm., 32:15-21.
1952. Reproductive and social behavior of the northern elephant
seal. Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool., 47:369-472.
1953. Behavioral factors affecting social structure in the Alaska fur
seal. Trans. 18th N. Amer. Wildl. Conf., Pp. 481-502.
1959. Mother-young relations and the maturation of pup behaviour
in the Alaska fur seal. Anim. Behav., 7:163-171.
BARTHOLOMEW, G. A., AND R. A. BooLooTiAN. 1960. Numbers and population
structure of the pinnipeds on the California Channel Islands. Jour.
Mamm., 41:366-375.
BARTHOLOMEWw, G. A., AND N. E. Coxiias. 1962. The role of vocalization in the
social behaviour of the northern elephant seal. Anim. Behav., 10:
7-14.
BARTHOLOMEW, G. A., AND P. G. Hoer. 1953. Reproductive behavior of the
Alaska fur seal, Callorhinus ursinus. Jour. Mamm., 34:417-436.
BARTHOLOMEW, G. A., AND C. L. Hupps. 1952. Winter population of pinnipeds
about Guadalupe, San Benito, and Cedros islands, Baja California.
Jour. Mamm., 33:160-171.
BARTHOLOMEWw, G. A., AND F. WILKE. 1956. Body temperature in the northern
fur seal, Callorhinus ursinus. Jour. Mamm., 37:327-337.
BERING SEA TRIBUNAL OF ARBITRATION. 1895. Fur seal arbitration: Proceedings
of the tribunal convened at Paris for the governments of the United
States and Great Britain, for determination of questions between the
two governments concerning jurisdictional rights to the waters of
the Bering Sea. Washington, D. C. 53rd Congress, 2nd Session. Senate
Ex. Doc. no. 177.
Bertram, G. C. L. 1940. The biology of the Weddell and crabeater seals, with
a study of the comparative behaviour of the Pinnipedia. British
Graham Land Exped., 1934-1937. Sci. Reps., 1:1-139.
Brest, A. D. 1961. The concept of ritualisation, Jn Current problems in ani-
mal behaviour. W. H. Thorpe and O. L. Zangwill (Eds.). University
Press, Cambridge, Pp. 102-124.
Bonnor, P. 1928. Report on the seals and sea lions of California. Fish. Bull. 14,
Calif. Div. Fish and Game., 62 pp.
Carrick, R., S. E. Csorpas, AND S. E. INGHAM. 1962. Studies on the southern
elephant seal, Mirounga leonina (L.) IV. Breeding and development.
Commonwealth Sci. Ind. Res. Org. Wildl. Res., 7:161—-197.
Cotiias, N. E. 1956. The analysis of socialization in sheep and goats. Ecology,
37228-2539.
Coutson, J. C. 1965. Colonial breeding in grey seals, In A seals symposium.
E. A. Smith (Ed.). The Nature Conservancy, Edinburgh, Scotland,
Pp. 55-56.
Coutson, J. C., ANp G. Hickiinc. 1964. The breeding biology of the grey seal,
73
74 The California Sea Lion
Halichoerus grypus (Fab.), on the Farne Islands, Northumberland.
Jour. Anim. Ecol., 33:485—512.
Dartz, G. M. 1959. Alaskan challengers of the sea. Nat. Hist., 68:334-347.
E1BL-EIBESFELDT, I. 1955. Ethologische Studien am Galapagos-Seeléwen, Zalo-
phus wollebaeki Sivertsen. Zeit. f. Tierpsychol., 12:286-303. (English
translation on file at Marine Mammal Biological Laboratory, Seattle.)
Evans, W. E., AnD R. M. Haucen. 1963. An experimental study of the echo-
location ability of a California sea lion, Zalophus californianus
(Lesson) . Bull. So. Cal. Acad. Sci., 62:165-175.
FerrEIRA, R. VAz. 1961. Estructura de una agrupacion social reproductora de
Otaria byronia (de Blainville), representacion grafica. Facultad de
Humanidades y Ciencias, Dept. Zool. Vertebrados, Univ. Montevideo,
Uruguay. Revista 19:253—260.
Fiscus, C. H., G. A. BAINES, AND F. WitkE. 1964. Pelagic fur seal investigations,
Alaska waters, 1962. U. S. Fish and Wildl. Serv., Washington, D. C.,
Spec. Sci. Rep. Fish. 475, 59 pp.
Fiscus, C. H., AND G. A. Baines. 1966. Food and feeding behavior of Steller
and California sea lions. Jour. Mamm., 47:195—200.
Fry, D. H., Jr. 1939. A winter influx of sea lions from Lower California.
Calif. Fish and Game, 25:245-250.
Hamitton, J. E. 1934. The southern sea lion, Otaria byronia (de Blainville) .
Discovery Reports, 8:269-318.
Harrison, R. J., L. H. MATTHEws, AND J. M. Roserts. 1952. Reproduction in
some Pinnipedia. Trans. Zool. Soc. London, 27:437-540, 4 pls.
Hewer, H. R. 1960. Behaviour of the grey seal (Halichoerus grypus Fab.) in
the breeding season. Mammalia, 24:400-421.
HILiincer, C. 1958. The California Islands. Academy Publications, Los Ange-
les, California, 165 pp.
Hosson, E. S. 1966. Visual orientation and feeding in seals and sea lions. Na-
ture, 210:326-327.
Howe Lt, A. B. 1930. Aquatic mammals, their adaptations to life in the water.
Chas. C Thomas, Baltimore, 338 pp.
Husss, C. L., AND G. I. RopeN. 1964. Oceanography and marine life along the
Pacific Coast of Middle America, In Vol. 1, Handbook of Middle
American Indians, University of Texas Press, Pp. 143-186.
Hux.ey, J. S. 1923. Courtship activities in the red-throated diver (Colymbus
stellatus Pontopp.); together with a discussion of the evolution of
courtship in birds. Jour. Linn. Soc. Lond., 35:253-292, 2 pls.
IrvInG, L., L. J. Peyton, C. H. BAHN, AND R. S. PETERSON. 1962. Regulation
of temperature in fur seals. Physiol. Zool., 35:275-284.
KENYON, K. W. 1956. The circus seal at home. Nat. Hist., 65:195-201.
1960. ‘Territorial behavior and homing in the Alaska fur seal. Mam-
malia, 24:432-444.
1962. History of the Steller sea lion at the Pribilof Islands, Alaska.
Jour. Mamm., 43:68-75.
Kenyon, K. W., AND D. W. Rice. 1959. Life history of the Hawaiian monk seal.
Pacific Science, 12:215-252.
Literature Cited 75
KENYON, K. W., AND F. WILKE. 1953. Migration of the northern fur seal,
Callorhinus ursinus. Jour. Mamm., 34:86—98.
KiNG, J. E. 1961. Notes on the pinnipedes from Japan described by Temminck
in 1844. Zool. Mededelingen, 37:211-224, 5 pls.
1964. Seals of the world. The British Museum (Natural History) ,
London, 154 pp.
Ktoprer, P. H., D. K. ApAmMs, AND M. S. KLoprer. 1964. Maternal “imprinting”
in goats. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 52:911—-914.
Laws, R. M. 1956. The elephant seal (Mirounga leonina, Linn.) II. General,
social and reproductive behaviour. Falkland Is. Dependencies Survey,
London. Sci. Rept., 13, 88 pp.
Loizos, C. 1966. Play in mammals. Symp. Zool. Soc. London, 18:1-9.
Lorenz, K. 1964. Ritualized fighting, Zn The natural history of aggression.
J. D. Carthy and F. J. Ebling (Eds.). Academic Press, New York,
Pp. 39-50.
Marter, P. 1963. Book review, The mountain gorilla: ecology and behavior,
by G. B. Schaller. Science, 140:1081-1082.
MatTHews, L. H. 1964. Overt fighting in mammals, Jn The natural history of
aggression. J. D. Carthy and F. J. Ebling (Eds.). Academic Press,
New York, Pp. 23-32.
MATHISEN, O. A., R. T. BAADE, AND R. J. Lopr. 1962. Breeding habits, growth
and stomach contents of the Steller sea lion in Alaska. Jour. Mamm.,
43 :469-477.
MEIGHAN, C. W., AND H. Esernart. 1953. Archaeological resources of San Nic-
olas Island, California. Amer. Antiquity, 19:109-125.
MITCHELL, Epwarp D., Jr. 1962. A walrus and a sea lion from the Pliocene
purisma formation at Santa Cruz, California: with remarks on the
type locality and geologic age of the sea lion Dusignathus santacruzen-
sis Kellogg. Los Angeles County Mus. Contrib. in Sci. 56, 23 pp.
NortH Paciric Fur SEAL COMMISSION, STANDING SCIENTIFIC COMMITTEE. 1962.
Report on investigations from 1958 to 1961. Washington, D. C., 183 pp.
1963. Glossary of terms used in fur seal research and management.
U. S. Dept. Interior, Fish and Wildl. Serv., Washington, D. C. Fishery
Leaflet 546, 9 pp.
Nice, M. M. 1962. Development of behavior in precocial birds. Trans. Linn.
SOG ING Noy Sct I IML,
Nuttinec, C. C. 1891. Some of the causes and results of polygamy among the
Pinnipedia. Amer. Naturalist, 25:103-112.
O’GorMAN, F. 1963. Observations on terrestrial locomotion in Antarctic seals.
Proc. Zool. Soc. London, 141:837-850.
Orr, R. T. 1965a. Interspecific behavior among pinnipeds. Zeit. f. Saiugetier-
kunde, 30:163-171.
1965b. Barrington Island. California Monthly, 75 (10) :18-20.
Orr, R. T., AND T. C. PouLTer. 1965. The pinniped population of Ano Nuevo
Island, California. Proc. Calif. Acad. Sciences, Fourth Series, 32:377—
404.
76 The California Sea Lion
PAULIAN, P. 1964. Contribution a l’etude de Votarie de lIle Amsterdam.
Mammalia, 28:1—146, 10 pls.
PETERSON, R. S. 1965a. Drugs for handling fur seals. Jour. Wildl. Megt., 29:
688-693.
1965b. Behavior of the northern fur seal. Dr. Sci. Thesis. Johns
Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, 214 pp.
PooLe, T. 1966. Aggressive play in polecats. Symp. Zool. Soc. London, 18:23—44.
Poutter, T. C. 1963. Sonar signals of the sea lion. Science, 139:753-755.
Ray, C. 1963. Locomotion in pinnipeds. Natural Hist., 72 (3) :10-21.
Row ey, J. 1929. Life history of the sea lions of the California coast. Jour.
Mamm., 10:1-36.
ScaAmMmon, C. M. 1874. The marine mammals of the north-western coast of
North America. John H. Carmany and Co., San Francisco, 319 pp.
SCHEFFER, V. B. 1958. Seals, sea lions and walruses. A review of the pinnipedia.
Stanford University Press. 179 pp., 33 pls.
1964. Hair patterns in seals (Pinnipedia). Jour. Morphol., 115:291—
303.
SCHEFFER, V. B., AND J. A. Nerr. 1948. Food of California sea lions. Jour.
Mamm., 29:67-68.
SCHUSTERMAN, R. J., AND S. N. FEINSTEIN. 1965. Shaping and discriminative con-
trol of underwater click vocalizations in a California sea lion. Science,
150:1743-1744.
SCHUSTERMAN, R. J., W. N. KELLocc, AND C. E. Rice. 1965. Underwater visual
discrimination by the California sea lion. Science, 147:1594-1596.
SHorT, R. V., AND J. M. Kinc. 1964. The design of a crossbow and dart for
immobilisation of wild animals. Vet. Record, 76:628-630.
SLIJPER, E. J. 1956. Some remarks on gestation and birth in cetacea and other
aquatic mammals. Hvalrad. Skr., 41:1-62.
SmitH, E. A. 1965. The effects of human interference in a grey seal breeding
colony and the phenomenon of fosterage, Jn A seals symposium. E. A.
Smith (Ed.). The Nature Conservancy, Edinburgh, Pp. 63-75.
SmiTH, M.S. R. 1965. Weddell seals, Jn A seals symposium. E. A. Smith (Ed.).
The Nature Conservancy, Edinburgh, Pp. 9-16.
STELLER, G. W. 1751. De _ bestiis marinis. ‘Typis Academiae Scientiarum,
Petropoli. Translation by W. Miller and J. E. Miller, In The fur
seals and fur-seal islands of the North Pacific Ocean, Part 3. D. S.
Jordan, et al. (Ed.) , 1898. Govt. Print. Office, Washington, Pp. 179-
218.
Watts, G. L. 1942. The vertebrate eye and its adaptive radiation. Cranbrook
Institute of Science, Bloomfield Hills, Michigan. Bull. No. 19, 785 pp.
WinsLow, J. H. 1960. San Nicolas Island, Channel Islands, Ventura County,
California, a library brochure. Prepared for Pacific Missile Range,
Point Mugu, California, with the University of California. 62 pp.
(Processed) .
WynneE-Epwarps, V. C. 1962. Animal dispersion in relation to social behavior.
Oliver and Boyd, Edinburgh, Scotland, 653 pp.
Accepted for publication August 15, 1966
Index
Aggressiveness:
bulls, 23-25
dimorphism related, 62
effects, 62
females, 29
functions, 59
ontogeny, 61
ritualization, 60
suppression, 63
transition to play, 19, 61
Alarm:
call, 39
learning, 20
reaction, 17, 19
Ano Nuevo Island, Calif., 47
Aquatic copulation, 34, 68
Arctocephalus gazella, 47, 66
Bark:
bull, 24
female, 30
Belch vocalization, female, 30
Bertram, G. C. L., 67-68
Birth, 31, 52
Boundary ceremony, 25, 60, Figs.
13-15
Breeding sites, characteristics, 21, 66
British Columbia, 4
Bull, definition, 2
California sea lion, see specific topic
Callorhinus ursinus, comparison
with Zalophus, see specific topic
Carrying of pup, female, 36
Chemical senses, 12
Copulation, 33, 52, Figs. 17-18
Corvus corax, 14
Cough, 39
Coulson, J. C., 68, 69
Courtship, 32
Crawl, 8
Cystophora, 67
Dates of breeding season, 52
Delivery of fetus, 31
Disturbance, see man
Dominance relations:
among females, 30
among non-breeders, 18
interspecific, 15
Drinking, sea water, 9
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I., 1, 6, 27, 30, 40
Elephant seal, see Mirounga
Engraulis, 5
Estrous display, 32
Estrus, 32
Eumetopias jubata, 14, 16, 67
Excretion, 9
Exploratory behavior, 39
Female aggregations, 27
Female:male ratio, 54—57
Fighting:
see also aggressiveness
bulls, 23, 25, 60
causes, 23
females, 29, 60
functions, 59
length, 24
Fiscus, C. H., 7
Flipper-waving, 10
Food, Zalophus, 5
Fosterage, 41
Fur seal, see Callorhinus or Arcto-
cephalus
Galapagos sea lion, see Zalophus
californianus wollebaeki
Gallop, 7, 37
Gregariousness:
females, 28
functions, 58
non-breeders, 18
Grooming, 8, Fig. 8
Growl, 30
Gull, see Larus
Halichoerus, 41, 67-69
Harbor seals, see Phoca
78 The California Sea Lion
Harem:
absence, 29
system, 68
use of word, 27
Hauling ground, definition, 2
Hearing, 12, 66
Hickling, G., 68
Huxley, J. S., 60
Hydrurga, 66
Immobilizing drugs and technique,
4
Individual recognition, see mater-
nal-young recognition
Intruders, territorial male, 25-26
Jug posture, 6
Kone a Eeyels
Land, time on, see periods of time
on land
Larus occidentalis, 14, 16
Leptonychotes weddelli, 5, 67
Lobodon carcinophagus, 8
Man, response to, 11, 13, 17, 18, 20,
21, 65-66, Fig. 9
Marking sea lions for study, 3, Fig.
Dy Werke, By, Vane a
Maternal-young recognition, 41
geography, 42
olfaction, 43
ontogeny, 41
vision, 43
vocalization, 42
Maternal-young relations:
first days, 36
maintenance, 41
post partum, 35
seasonal change, 44
separations, 37
specificity, 41, Fig. 5
Mazatlan, Mexico, 4
Measurements, Zalophus, 4
Merluccius, 5
Migration, seasonal, 4, 47
Mirounga angustirostris, 14, 15, 30,
41, 64
Model, evolutionary, 68
Monachus, 14, 67
Mortality, land, 49
Mother-response call, 35, 38, 42
Musky odor, bulls, 13
Neck-rubbing, 33
Neophoca, 67
Nocturnal behavior, 11, 27, 28, 65
Nursing, 37, 40, 67
Nutting, C. C., 67
Oblique stare, 25, Fig. 14
Observations:
methods, 2
nocturnal, 3
O’Gorman, F., 8
Olfaction, 13, 42, 64
Ontogeny, behavioral, 37
Orr, R. T., 16, 44
Otaria, 12, 67
Paulian, P., 10
Peace-keeping by bulls, 30
Periods of time on land:
bulls, 23, 49
females, 49
functions, 63
pups, 52
suckling, 54
timing, 53
Phalacorcorax pencillatus, 14
Phocarctos, 67
Phoca, 12, 14, 15, 66
Play, 6, 19, 39, Figs. 19-20
transition to fights, 61
with inanimate objects, 40
Pods, pups, 38
Population:
fluctuations, 45
estimates, San Nicolas I., 45, 47
Porpoising, 6
Polygyny, 21, 28, 54-57, 66-68
Precociousness, pups, 35
Pregnant females, 29, 31
Pup-attraction call, 31, 35, 42
Pup, definition, 2
Range, geograhic, Zalophus, 4
Regurgitation, 9
Ritualized behavior, 60
Rookery:
definition, 2
determinants of location, 21, 66
Rowley, J., 1, 44
San Benitos, Islas, Mexico, 44
SanmeeNicolas le Calif; 2; Fig. 1,
Fig. 23
San Miguel I., Calif., 4, 16
Scammon, C. M., 1
Sex ratio, 54—57
Sexual segregation, seasonal, 4
Shoreline territories, 22
Site tenacity, 21, 66
Sleeping postures, 8, 38, Figs. 6
and 7
Spatial distribution:
breeding season, 22, 27, Fig. 12
seasonal changes, 47
Squeal, female, 30
Steller, G. W., 31
Steller sea lion, see Eumetopias
Stride, 8
Subordinate breeding males, 27
Suckling, 37, 40, 44, 54, Fig. 21
Swimming, 6, 19
copulation during, 34
lessons, 37
pups, 37
Tactile sense, 13, 43
‘Taste sense, 13, 43
Terrestrial locomotion, 7, 64
Territorial bull, definition, 2
Territories of bulls, 22, Fig. 10
aquatic, 22, Fig. 11
boundaries, 22
duration, 49
Index 79
establishment, 23
intruders, 25
maintenance, 25
size, 22
Thermoregulation, 10, 22, 28, 37,
58, 65
Threats:
aggressive, 59
play, 19
open-mouthed,
Fig. 16
Thigmotaxis, 14, 18
Tides, relation of rookery to, 22, 27
eh BS), Gill, HY).
Urocyon littoralis, 14
Vibrissae, 13, 25
Vision, 11, 26, 42, 64
Vocalization:
bulls, 24
females, 30, 42
nocturnal, 28
ontogeny, 35
pups, 38, 42
ritualized, 61
significance, 65
Wandering bulls, 25
Walk, 7, 37
Weather, responses to, 9
Weights, Zalophus, 4
Whisker-stropping, 9
Wind, responses to, 10, 21
Wynne-Edwards, V. C., 68
Yearling, behavior, 44, Fig. 22
Zalophus californianus, see also spe-
cific topic
californianus, 4
wollebaeki, 1, 4, 17, 40
japonicus, 4
ei
emPyEY (od |
™ . Peitahite
OS yee Behe
e
oy a
Ra re pe pry ir
54)
im
Pua
sie:
ERNST MAYR LIBRARY
wii
93 303
Hi Hil it a] titeat
a
at
i Hal
Pens SSS SSS
BSSSRISS
i Hi i
a
a 7 | 0
ii ii
i a es
| : i a
/
ee epee, — = =
ie
ae
i 7 H
i He li
a a 7
th 7
ee =
=
| RUBIO na ‘
a ue | i
| : a a
i inh MH AIRGEM EH i
a : a 7
a a ) : 7
HU a Hi Hh
a | i 1 ce a ii -
ci
oe
i a a i a Ht i a mee
iy i i i Hi Oi Hf it un nih
if : . | a - i a . 7
HN i Bi ah At oo
it i iit ce aM oo ae
a
| if |
PAD
ah
i a sii oe i : i
eit HI i ii
; i a i i \ ah on NY
ny Di a {
ae ie Ha a ae H
a i uit ii va es ii ni
ji a i Mi a if of :
4
a i uy
7 7 a iM i Heit
L . a bai | :
ae a a \ i
a i: aan
Vs Hh 7 | i
7 i i th ,
oe : 1 : .
:
i
i nit Ri / . o mn
a we aah ‘ _ hi . a
- a Hi ae a a | it
mein) ii ) a. u IGS Ha | i in Hut } iy Hi
Hi Hh i oF Ha ia a i au DOA ATTN nh if
i ‘ | a i i 7 7 7 ' : . a oe 7
i etealiie att \
aa a . ' ae
ao
a a
Saas
Sa
a = a
SS SS
Ls
eee
SS
ee
ees eee
= HS
Fe SE
SSS ee
SSS
= = : Pe
eee
SSS eee aS SS